Summary

Examining Bilingual Language Control Using the Stroop Task

Published: February 26, 2020
doi:

Summary

This bilingual Stroop task uses Congruent, Incongruent, and Neutral stimuli presented in blocks in the first language (L1) only, the second language (L2) only, and a combination of L1 and L2. This task allows for an examination of language processing and cognitive control in both L1 and L2.

Abstract

The Stroop task in its many variations has been used in fields such as psychology, linguistics, and neuroscience to examine questions regarding the automaticity of reading, language processing, and cognitive control, among others. When looking at bilingual individuals, this task can be used to obtain measures of language interference and control in both a bilingual's first language (L1) and second language (L2), as well as for testing the bilingual advantage hypothesis. The Stroop task presents participants with color terms written in congruent colors (e.g., the word RED written in red font), incongruent colors (e.g., the word RED written in green font), in addition to noncolor terms for control (e.g., the word TREE presented in any color), and uses the reaction times from the different conditions to assess the degree of interference and facilitation. In the covert version of the Stroop bilingual task (i.e., participants respond by pressing a button rather than naming aloud), stimuli in the L1 and the L2 are typically presented in separate blocks. While this allows for a simple, yet effective assessment of processing and cognitive control in each language, it fails to capture any potential differences in processing and control within bilingual young adult groups. The present task combines single-language blocks with a novel mixed-language block to increase the level of difficulty of the task, thus making it suitable for testing cognitive control in young adults. Representative results showing differences between performance in the single-language vs. mixed-language blocks are presented, and the benefits of a mixed-language block are discussed.

Introduction

The Stroop task, named after its creator John Ridley Stroop, has enjoyed over 80 years of popularity in the literature1. This simple task has been used in hundreds of studies, with different variants created to examine diverse populations and research questions in fields such as psychology, linguistics, and neuroscience. In particular, it has allowed researchers to examine language processes such as the automaticity of written word reading2, as well as associated cognitive control processes. The latter are also termed "executive control", and encompass a set of processes that include, but are not limited to, inhibition (i.e., interference suppression), attention, conflict monitoring and resolution, selection, and task switching. Looking specifically at bilinguals, the task is highly suitable for obtaining measures of language interference and control in both a bilingual's first language (L1) and second language (L2), as well as for testing the bilingual advantage hypothesis3, which is currently a topic of considerable debate.4

In the original task, the colors red, green, blue, brown, and purple were used in a series of three experiments. In the first experiment, participants read aloud a list of color words printed in incongruent colors (e.g., the word PURPLE printed in blue ink; correct answer "purple") and the same list printed in black ink. In the second experiment, participants said aloud the color of the words in the list (e.g., the word PURPLE printed in blue ink; correct answer "blue") and also named the same colors presented simply as colored squares. Finally, the third experiment examined whether practice of these tasks would affect the outcome, measured in all cases as the time required to read or to name the items in a given list.

The results revealed an interesting asymmetry between word-reading and color-naming: the difference between reading words in different colors and in black was a nonsignificant 2.3 seconds (or a 5.6% increase in the time required to read the color words), while the difference between naming the colors in which incongruent color words were written and naming the colors of squares was a 47.0 seconds, or a significant 74.3% increase in naming time for the words1. In other words, colors did not interfere with word reading, but reading strongly interfered with color naming. This increase in color naming time in the presence of a conflicting written word has come to be known as the Stroop effect, and while practice can reduce its magnitude, the interference cannot be altogether eliminated.

Different theories have been proposed to account for the Stroop effect, and in an extensive review of 50 years of Stroop literature, MacLeod5 describes two of the more predominant ones: 1) the relative speed of processing and 2) selective attention. In the former, words are read faster than colors are named, and this difference in relative processing time causes the Stroop effect. In the latter, the controlled, resource-intensive process of color naming occurs in parallel with the automatic process of reading; the direct competition between diverging word and color information is the source of interference and the Stroop effect2. The theory of selective attention is currently the more accepted view6,7,8,9.

Resolving the word-color competition is a cognitively demanding process that requires inhibiting distracting information while turning attention to the goal of the task. In order to correctly name the color, this interference from the automatically processed written word must be suppressed, while attention must be turned to the less practiced, and therefore controlled, task of color naming. Thus, the Stroop task becomes a measure of not only inhibition, but also of attention, and through various manipulations, it allows different levels of cognitive control to be examined10. Typically, a decreased magnitude of Stroop interference is considered indicative of better inhibition and attentional resource allocation. A full theoretical review of the Stroop task is beyond the scope of the current paper, but is available from previous works2,5. Although nonlinguistic versions of the Stroop task exist (e.g., numerical11, oculomotor12, spatial13), the current work is interested in linguistic cognitive control, and the remainder of the discussion will therefore be focused on the original linguistic version of the task.

The original Stroop task has undergone various modifications since 1935, and the task commonly used now includes a congruent condition where the color word is presented in the color it names (the word BLUE presented in blue ink; correct answer, "blue") and a control condition that can be made up of colored shapes as in the original task, asterisks (***), a row of symbols such as X's, %'s or #'s, or high-frequency, noncolor-related words (e.g., DOG). These additions allow the Stroop task to examine facilitation effects. Facilitation effects are brought about by the convergence of information from the written word and the visual color in the congruent condition and thereby speed up naming times7 (although see MacLeod5 for reliability issues regarding facilitation effects). They can also be used to obtain baseline reading time or color naming time measures using the control items, in addition to Stroop interference effects. Facilitation and interference are defined as the difference in response time between the congruent and control, and as the difference between the incongruent and control trials, respectively, although the Stroop effect can sometimes be calculated as the difference between the congruent and incongruent trial, as in the original task6. In the task, the congruent, incongruent, and neutral conditions can be presented in individual blocks (all congruent, all incongruent, or all neutral trials) or in a single block made up of all three conditions, and in a variety of languages.

In the bilingual Stroop task, color terms in the bilingual's first language (L1) or second language (L2), or both, may be presented, and participants may be instructed to name the colors in either of their two languages. This type of task allows researchers to examine both within-language interference (by having the participant name L1 words in the L1 and L2 words in the L2) and between-language interference (naming L1 words in the L2, and vice versa), giving greater insight into how bilinguals process and manage their languages. For example, between-language interference can tell us about lexical representation and the strength of connections between languages in the mental lexicon, and this interference is typically smaller than within-language interference14.

A variety of language pairs have been used for the Stroop task's bilingual adaptation, including Chinese-English15,16, Japanese-English15,17,18, Spanish-English15,19,20, French-Arabic21, Arabic-Hebrew22, English-Greek14, English-German14, and Dutch-English7. As some have noted15, factors such as orthography in these languages can influence the magnitude of the within- and between-language interference. Still, despite differences in magnitude, the Stroop effect is always apparent.

It is important to note that many of the studies that incorporate two languages simultaneously20 require overt naming, which is susceptible to lexical access effects23. In tasks requiring a button press, the L1 and L2 are typically presented separately24, and only include three or four colors6,8. However, such a presentation may make the task too simple for young adults, whose cognitive control abilities are at their peak25, and result in all participants performing at ceiling. Thus, a single-language presentation with a small number of possible responses may not be sufficient to elicit any possible differences between groups of young adult bilinguals. Therefore, the current protocol seeks to increase the task challenge by increasing the number of color terms and mixing both the L1 and L2 items in a single block, thus not only examining reading automaticity, attention, and cognitive control processes, but also creating a task that is suitable for testing bilingual cognitive control in the young adult population.

In this bilingual task, stimuli were selected in English and French. An example of our stimuli in used is in Figure 1. However, any two languages can be used. For this reason, the protocol below will simply use Language A (La) and Language B (Lb) to describe stimuli in each language. For full details of the experimental protocol, please refer to our previous work26,27.

Protocol

All methods and procedures described here have been approved by the University of Ottawa Research Ethics Board.

Figure 1
Figure 1: Stroop task sample stimuli. The color and noncolor terms as well as the background color used in the current experiment are shown. For Incongruent and Neutral stimuli, sample color-word stimuli are shown. In the experiment, any word could be in any of the six colors. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

1. Prepare the stimuli for the bilingual Stroop task

NOTE: The Stroop task can be programmed in many commercially available software packages designed for the presentation of behavioral experiments (see Table of Materials)

  1. Create the Congruent items in Language a (La) by programming six color terms written in capital letters in size 60 Times New Roman font, for which the written word and the font color match. For example, present the word GREEN in green font, and the word RED in red font.
  2. Create the Incongruent items in La by programming six color terms written in capital letters in size 60 Times New Roman font, for which the written word and the font color do not match. For example, present the word GREEN in red font, and the word RED in green font.
  3. Create the Neutral items in La by programming six noncolor and noncolor-associated terms, written in capital letters in size 60 Times New Roman font. Words are used instead of symbols in order to elicit baseline reading speeds for this reading-based task.
    1. Present each word in each of the colors used in the previous two steps. For example, present the word BOOK in both green and red font.
  4. Repeat the previous three steps to create items in Language b (Lb). Ensure that you do not use color terms that are cognates in the two languages (e.g., the word BLUE and BLEU in English and French, respectively.)
    NOTE: In the current experiment, La was English, and the color terms used were RED, YELLOW, GREEN, BLACK, SILVER, and WHITE. Lb was French, and the color terms used were ROUGE, JAUNE, VERT, NOIR, ARGENT, and BLANC.

2. Create two single-language blocks

  1. Create an La block which includes 25 Congruent, 25 Incongruent, and 25 Neutral items, for a total of 75 items in the block. Include five Neutral practice items at the beginning of the block.
  2. Program the experiment to present all items in randomized intermixed trials as shown in Figure 2.

Figure 2
Figure 2: Stroop task procedure. An incongruent trial is shown. Trials began with a 250 ms fixation cross and ended when the participant pressesed a response button or 4,000 ms elapsed. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

  1. Repeat the previous two steps with 25 Congruent, 25 Incongruent, and 25 Neutral items in Lb, for a total of 75 items in the block.
  2. Include a self-timed break between the two blocks, and a self-timed break before the presentation of the mixed-language block described in Part 3 below.

3. Create one mixed-language block

  1. Create a block which includes the 25 Congruent, 25 Incongruent, and 25 Neutral items in La, and the 25 Congruent, 25 Incongruent, and 25 Neutral items in Lb, for a total of 150 items in the block. Include five Neutral practice items at the beginning of the block.
  2. Program the experiment to present all items in randomized intermixed trials as shown in Figure 2.

4. Test participants

  1. Test participants in a sound-attenuated testing room or booth. Participants should be seated comfortably in front of a computer screen with a button box or keyboard in front of them.
  2. Explain to the participants that they will see words in different colors on the screen in front of them, and that their task is to press the button that matches the color of the text that they see. Emphasize that they should answer as quickly and accurately as possible, but that it is okay if they make a mistake. Allow time for participants to ask questions.
  3. Once participants are comfortable with the task, leave the testing room/booth and allow them to begin the experiment. The experiment will take approximately 8 min to complete.
  4. At the end of the experiment, thank the participants for their time and debrief them about the task and its purpose.

Representative Results

One benefit of including both single-language blocks as well as a mixed-language blocks is that it is possible to confirm the expected results (facilitation and inhibition effects) in each of the participants' languages. It will then be possible to interpret the findings from the mixed-language block. The results presented below are from a study investigating English-French Bilinguals. One of our main research questions focuses on how the age at which a second language is learned (age of acquisition, or AoA) may affect linguistic and general cognitive processing. Thus, here we are using our previously examined AoA groups for illustrative purposes to show different benefits of bilingualism based on AoA. These data include a group of Simultaneous Bilinguals, who acquired both English and French from birth, and a group of Early Bilinguals who acquired English as a first language and were then immersed in French as a second language in an early immersion school program. These participants were exposed to and immersed in French before the age of 7. The participants all reported currently using both English and French on a daily basis and the groups did not differ significantly on proficiency measures.

The first analysis conducted was to examine whether or not each of the participant groups showed the expected facilitation and inhibition effects in each of their languages in the single-language blocks. As can be seen in Figure 3 below, each of the participant groups showed both facilitation and inhibition effects. However, using only the single-language blocks there was no reason to claim any differences between the Simultaneous and Early Bilinguals. Thus, these two different types of bilinguals are often grouped together for analyses, which may be partly the cause of some of the findings of no cognitive control differences between bilinguals and monolinguals.

Figure 3
Figure 3: Results from the single-language blocks. Main effects of congruency (facilitation and inhibition) were seen for each of the participant groups. No significant differences across groups were seen. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

However, when we looked at the same effects but in the mixed-language block (Figure 4), each language group showed differing effects of facilitation and inhibition. Once the task was made more challenging, between-group differences started to emerge. Different patterns of facilitation and inhibition effects were found, and we can no longer support an argument to collapse across Simultaneous and Early Bilingual participant groups. A mixed-language block likely causes language switching effects, which may result in cognitive costs for some participants. Interestingly, this may allow us to also look specifically at switching costs between La and Lb. However, a discussion of this is beyond the scope of the current paper.

Figure 4
Figure 4: Results from the mixed-language blocks. The Simultaneous Bilinguals showed a significant inhibition effect for the English trials while the Early Bilinguals showed a strong facilitation effect for the English trials and a strong inhibition effect for the French trials. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

Discussion

The experimental design presented here describes a twist on the traditional Stroop task. The main goal of this twist is to add a level of complexity to the task that may allow differences to emerge between groups that, due to their age, are being tested at their peak of performance. Essentially, to make the task more challenging in order to be able to distinguish between groups, we added a mixed-language block to the traditional Stroop task, which typically only collects data from trials in one language at a time. For this variant to work, researchers must ensure that critical stimuli do not include cognate items and that control words do not evoke specific colors (e.g., do not use "grass", due to its association with the color green). One other issue that has not been addressed in the current task is whether any effect of language mode28 plays a role. We have not currently controlled for the language used by the experimenter with the participant: all participants were tested in English, which was not necessarily the dominant language of all participants. Thus, controlling for language mode is likely to be an important addition in future research. Furthermore, similar to all variants of the linguistic Stroop task, cognitive control is tested through visually-presented language. Therefore, if the task is used to test bilingual cognitive control abilities, it is likely necessary to also employ nonlinguistic tasks of cognitive control. Controlling for language mode and testing nonlinguistic cognition may provide additional comparisons that demonstrate a more realistic pattern of cognitive benefits that result from bilingualism.

As the results indicate, the expected results of facilitation to congruent trials and inhibition to incongruent trials were evident (see Figure 3) when we tested participants in a task where only one language was required. No differences were found between the Simultaneous Bilinguals and the Early Bilinguals. This result is consistent with the data found in other Stroop tasks testing a single language at a time8. However, it is important to note that these cross-group similarities were no longer seen when a mixed-language task was given in this experiment. This task is likely more difficult for participants and this increased difficulty caused differences to emerge in the behavioral data. In fact, in another study that used both a single and a mixed-language condition in a non-Stroop conflict resolution task, differences across the conditions were found, with greater executive control functions found during the mixed-language conditions29.

As we have stated elsewhere26, the different patterns of facilitation and inhibition found in the mixed-language Stroop task suggest that even though both Simultaneous and Early Bilingual participants acquired both of their languages early, there are likely different underlying processes linked to each language. Importantly, this means that these participants should not be grouped together in tasks testing their language and even more general cognitive skills. Additionally, while these differences found in the mixed-language blocks could be simply due to the added complexity, it is also worth considering the effect of language context. Bilinguals are a very heterogeneous group who use their languages in very different ways, and thus English-French bilinguals in a bilingual city (e.g., Ottawa, Canada) may perform differently from English-French bilinguals from a city with less bilingualism in the languages of the participant (e.g., Toronto, Canada). These context effects (see Green and colleagues30,31) will need to be further explored with careful consideration of both past and current usage of both languages for bilingual participants. However, it must be noted that with the behavioral differences that are found, it is not clear what underlying processes cause these cross-group effects. The future directions of this type of research will be to explore what happens at the neurological level by measuring behavioral data concurrently with brain data (e.g., event-related brain potentials).

Offenlegungen

The authors have nothing to disclose.

Acknowledgements

The research presented here was supported by a SSHRC Standard Research Grant and a SSHRC Insight Grant to the 1st author and by a SSHRC Doctoral Fellowship and an Ontario Graduate Fellowship to the 2nd author. We wish to thank the members of the ERPLing lab for discussion of the data and for help with the testing of the participants.

Materials

Button box Cedrus Button box for response; however, any response pad or the computer keyboard can be used to collect responses.
Desktop computer (Windows OS) Dell Computer system for delivering stimuli; however, any computer, including laptops, can be used.
Presentation Neurobehavioural Systems Software for presenting behavioural experiments; however, the experiment can be programmed using a variety of experimental software.

Referenzen

  1. Stroop, J. R. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 18, 643-662 (1935).
  2. MacLeod, C. M., MacDonald, P. A. Interdimensional interference in the Stroop effect: uncovering the cognitive and neural anatomy of attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 4 (10), 383-391 (2000).
  3. Bialystok, E. The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin. 143, 233-262 (2017).
  4. Vīnerte, S., Sabourin, L. Reviewing the bilingual cognitive control literature: can a brain-based approach resolve the debate. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology. 73 (2), 118-134 (2019).
  5. MacLeod, C. M. Half a century of research in the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin. 109 (2), 163-203 (1991).
  6. Bialystok, E., Craik, F., Luk, G. Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 34 (4), 859-873 (2008).
  7. Roelofs, A. Attention and facilitation: converging information versus inadvertent reading in Stroop task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 36 (2), 411-422 (2010).
  8. Kousaie, S., Phillips, N. A. Aging and bilingualism: Absence of a “bilingual advantage” in Stroop interference in a nonimmigrant sample. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 65 (2), 356-369 (2012).
  9. Coderre, E. L., Van Heuven, W. J. B., Conklin, K. The timing and magnitude of Stroop interference and facilitation in monolinguals and bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 16 (2), 420-441 (2013).
  10. Bugg, J. M. Dissociating levels of cognitive control: The case of Stroop interference. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 21 (5), 302-309 (2012).
  11. Hernandez, M., Costa, A., Fuentes, L. F., Vivas, A. B., Sebastian-Galles, N. The impact of bilingualism on the executive control and orienting networks of attention. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 13 (3), 315-325 (2010).
  12. Singh, N., Mishra, R. K. Does language proficiency modulate oculomotor control? Evidence from Hindi-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 15, 771-781 (2012).
  13. Blumenfeld, H. K., Marian, V. Cognitive control in bilinguals: advantages in stimulus-stimulus inhibition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 17 (3), 610-629 (2014).
  14. Brauer, M., Healy, A. F., Bourne, L. E. Stroop interference in bilinguals: The role of similarity between the two languages. Foreign language learning: Psycholinquistic studies on training and retention. , 317-337 (1998).
  15. Fang, S., Ovid, J. L. T., Alva, L. Intralanguage vs. interlanguage Stroop effects in two types of writing systems. Memory and Cognition. 9 (6), 609-617 (1981).
  16. Coderre, E., Van Heuven, W. J. B. Electrophysiological Explorations of the Bilingual Advantage: Evidence from a Stroop Task. PLoS ONE. 9 (7), 1-15 (2014).
  17. Sumiya, H., Healy, H. Phonology in the bilingual Stroop effect. Memory and Cognition. 32, 752-758 (2004).
  18. Vīnerte, S. . Effects of bilingualism on cognitive control: considering the age of immersion and different linguistic environments. , (2018).
  19. Roselli, M., et al. Stroop effect in Spanish-English bilinguals. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society. 8, 819-827 (2002).
  20. Naylor, L. J., Stanley, E. M., Wicha, N. Y. Y. Cognitive and electrophysiological correlates of the bilingual Stroop effect. Frontiers in Psychology. 3 (81), 1-16 (2012).
  21. Zied, K. M., et al. Bilingualism and adult differences in inhibitory mechanisms: Evidence from a bilingual Stroop task. Brain and Cognition. 54, 254-256 (2004).
  22. Tzelgov, J., Henik, A., Leiser, D. Controlling Stroop interference: Evidence from a bilingual task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 16, 760-771 (1990).
  23. Pelham, S. D., Abrams, L. Cognitive advantages and disadvantages for early and late bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 40 (2), 313-325 (2014).
  24. Heidlmayr, K., et al. Successive bilingualism and executive functions: The effect of second language use on inhibitory control in a behavioural Stroop Color Word task. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 17 (3), 630-645 (2014).
  25. Costa, A., Hernández, M., Sebastián-Gallés, N. Bilingualism aids in conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition. 106, 59-86 (2008).
  26. Sabourin, L., Vīnerte, S. The bilingual advantage in the Stroop task: simultaneous vs. early bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 18 (2), 350-355 (2015).
  27. Sabourin, L., Vīnerte, S. Cognitive control among immersed bilinguals: Considering differences in linguistic and nonlinguistic processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 22 (3), 590-605 (2019).
  28. Grosjean, F. Studying bilinguals: Methodological and conceptual issues. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 1 (2), 131-149 (1998).
  29. Wu, Y. J., Thierry, G. Fast modulation of executive function by language context in bilinguals. Journal of Neuroscience. 33 (33), 13533-13537 (2013).
  30. Green, D. W. Language control in different contexts: The behavioral ecology of bilingual speakers. Frontiers in Psychology. 2 (103), 1-4 (2011).
  31. Green, D. W., Abutalebi, J. Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 25 (5), 515-530 (2013).

Play Video

Diesen Artikel zitieren
Sabourin, L., Vīnerte, S. Examining Bilingual Language Control Using the Stroop Task. J. Vis. Exp. (156), e60479, doi:10.3791/60479 (2020).

View Video