JoVE Educazione Scientific
Experimental Psychology
È necessario avere un abbonamento a JoVE per visualizzare questo.  Accedi o inizia la tua prova gratuita.
JoVE Educazione Scientific Experimental Psychology
Ethics in Psychology Research
  • 00:00Panoramica
  • 00:59Experimental Design
  • 02:43Running the Experiment
  • 05:01Representative Results
  • 05:33Applications
  • 06:55Summary

Ética en la investigación de la psicología

English

Condividere

Panoramica

Fuente: Laboratorios de Gary Lewandowski, Dave Strohmetz y Natalie Ciarocco — Universidad de Monmouth

Cuando un investigador encuentra un tema interesante para estudiar como la agresión, el objetivo se suele estudiar de una manera que es como fiel a la vida como sea posible. Sin embargo, los investigadores deben actuar de manera ética.  Para ello, deben equilibrar sus objetivos de investigación con los mejores intereses de los participantes. Ética a menudo entra en el proceso de planificación cuando los investigadores a identificar todas las formas que pueden manipular o medir una variable, pero haga su decisión final basándose en cómo debe manipular o medir una variable.

Después de recibir una calificación pobre en un papel o prueba, un estudiante universitario puede aparecer tomar hacia fuera (es decir, actuar en forma agresiva hacia) sus compañeros por ser malo o desagradable, gritar, tirar cosas o incluso llegar a ser físicamente violentos. La agresión es un comportamiento humano importante para estudiar y comprender debido a las implicaciones que tiene para la violencia interpersonal. Sin embargo, por razones de seguridad, un estudio no puede exponer a los participantes el riesgo que presenta tipos graves de violencia. Como resultado, los investigadores deben identificar similar pero benignos comportamientos que nos ayudan a entienden comportamientos más agresivos, sin dañar los participantes.

Este vídeo utiliza un experimento de dos grupos para ver si la gente realmente saca su enojo a los demás aunque los demás no son responsables por el problema original. Específicamente, analiza si retroalimentación negativa conduce a la agresión mientras que teniendo en cuenta cuestiones éticas tales como daño a los participantes, los costes vs beneficios, consentimiento informado y la información.

Estudios psicológicos suelen ser de mayores tamaños de muestra que los estudios en otras ciencias. Un gran número de los participantes ayuda a asegurar mejor que la población en estudio está mejor representada, es decir, el margen de error acompañado por estudiar el comportamiento humano se explica suficientemente. En este video se demuestra este experimento utilizando a sólo dos participantes, uno de ellos el evaluador. Sin embargo, según lo representado en los resultados, se utilizó un total de 245 participantes para llegar a conclusiones del experimento.

Procedura

1. definir el comportamiento ético en la investigación. La ética es un conjunto de normas morales y principios que guían las decisiones que tomemos. Esencialmente nos dicen lo que debemos hacer. Qué podría hacer el investigador es diferente de lo que deben hacer. Análisis de costo-beneficio: Para saber que si la investigación debe llevarse a cabo, el investigador debe asegurarse de que los beneficios superan los costos o riesgos de daño.  Esto puede lograrse aumentando los beneficios de lo…

Risultati

The data were collected from 245 participants. Recall that the aggression scale was calculated on the number assigned to each of the drinks, which varied in levels of distasteful flavor. A t-test for independent means was run to compare the negative and neutral feedback conditions to determine how they influenced aggression. The results indicated that participants who received the negative feedback generally chose more noxious drinks for the innocent person in the other room (the friend of the evaluator), which is an indication of aggression (Figure 1).

Figure 1
Figure 1. Amount of aggression by feedback condition.

Applications and Summary

This two-group experiment shows how researchers can study sensitive topics in an ethical way that minimizes harm to participants, while still allowing participants to engage in an aggressive behavior.

As they study human behavior, psychologists often seek to analyze undesirable and troubling behavior. For example a recent study in Psychology of Popular Media Culture found that when video-game players lost a game, they were more likely to act aggressively by trash-talking.1 Though aggressive, this behavior is less risky than physical aggression and is common, which shows the researchers considered the ethical implications of their research.

Ethics apply beyond research. When considering ethical dilemmas in everyday life, there often is not a clear right or wrong answer. Should we test cosmetics on animals? Should Facebook be allowed to change how information appears on a user’s page to see if it changes the user’s behavior? The issues are complicated, but it is imperative that researchers consider these issues and seek out ways to answer their research questions in ways that protect participants.

Riferimenti

  1. Harmon-Jones, E., & Sigelman, J. State anger and prefrontal brain activity: Evidence that insult-related relative left-prefrontal activation is associated with experienced anger and aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 80 797-803 (2001).
  2. Breuer, J., Scharkow, M., & Quandt, T. Sore losers? A reexamination of the frustration–aggression hypothesis for collocated video game play. Psychology of Popular Media Culture. (2013).

Trascrizione

Sensitive topics in research require careful planning to uphold ethical behavior- the moral standards that guide decision-making.

Designing studies in an ethical manner requires a balancing act between the benefits of the research and the costs or risk of harm to participants.

This decision process is referred to as a cost-benefit analysis, in which the study’s intent outweighs the high costs or risks of harm for those involved.

By applying ethical principles, this video demonstrates how to design, perform, analyze, and interpret an experiment about interpersonal aggression. Importantly, researchers study anger towards others without resorting to physical harm by incorporating more benign forms of aggressive behavior.

For this experiment, consider two sensitive topics, negative feedback and aggression, that require cost-benefit analyses to demonstrate ethical compliance.

Negative feedback towards participants might entail a number of different forms, including: medical results that indicate disease, a diagnostic test that indicates low IQ, harsh commentary on physical appearance, or severe criticism on written work.

Aggression could involve a number of behaviors, such as being verbally abusive to the participant, physically pushing the participant, administering an electrical shock to the participant, or giving the participant a foul-tasting drink.

Here, the experiment will focus on providing severe criticism on participant’s written work.

Using a two-group design, all participants write a paragraph about a day at the beach. One group receives negative feedback in the form of negative comments, whereas the second group receives neutral feedback, or no comments.

After receiving criticism, participants are asked to choose a beverage for their paragraph evaluator’s friend. The beverage choice correlates to the level of aggression displayed by the participant.

The hypothesis of the experiment is negative feedback induces aggression that would be taken out on another individual.

Thus, those who receive negative comments are expected to retaliate and choose more distasteful drink choices than those who receive no comments.

To conduct the experiment, gather the informed consent and final debriefing papers, a black pen, and a blank piece of paper. In a different room, you will need: dice, a red pen, index cards, 5 cups of water, a tray or platter, and portions of sugar, lemon juice, vinegar, and hot sauce.

To begin the experiment, meet the participant in the lab. Guide all participants through the consent process and discuss the overall plan for the session.

With the participant sitting at a desk, ask them to write a brief paragraph that describes a day at the beach. After the participant finishes, inform them that another researcher will evaluate the paragraph over the next 5 min.

Once in another room, roll dice to randomly determine the kind of feedback the participant receives. Assign negative feedback for an even number, and write comments on the paragraph with a red pen. If the dice roll results in an odd number, assign neutral feedback, and do not make any marks on the page.

After providing feedback, return the paragraph to the participant. Suggest that they read over the comments when you leave the room to set-up the next phase of the experiment.

While the participant waits, prepare five different beverages that range from pleasant to unpleasant tastes: highly sugared water, lemon water, plain water, vinegar in water, and hot sauce in water.

Then, label five different index cards with a number on one side and description on the other. Arrange each pair on a platter.

After arranging the platter, carry it into the room with the participant. Explain what each beverage contains. Instruct them to choose one beverage for the evaluator’s friend to drink in the other room. Record the number associated with the chosen beverage.

At the conclusion of the experiment, debrief participants and explain why deception was necessary for the experiment.

To analyze how aggressive behavior is expressed after feedback, average the numbers recorded for the chosen beverages in each condition.

The data are then graphed by plotting the mean number in each condition. In this experiment, participants who received negative feedback retaliated and chose a more distasteful beverage for the evaluator’s friend than those in the neutral group who did not receive any feedback.

Now that you are familiar with how psychologists study sensitive topics in an ethical way, let’s take a look at how other researchers are mindful of moral standards that promote safe alternatives for studying troubling and undesirable behaviors.

A recent study found that when video game players lost a game, they were more likely to act aggressively by “trash-talking.” The researcher considered the ethical implications of the design because trash-talking is less risky than physical aggression.

This study uses a social test that reliably induces a stress response in participants.

Physiological measures, such as skin conductance, heart rate and stress hormone levels, are obtained non-invasively through simple monitoring equipment and saliva samples.

Thus, this experiment provides an ethical alternative to painful physical stressors like treadmill running or cold pressor test.

Facing numerous ethical concerns, animal researchers use: within-group designs to reduce the number of unnecessary subjects, behavioral tasks to obtain ethological responses, and administer pharmaceutical agents to minimize pain and suffering.

You’ve just watched JoVE’s introduction to Ethics in Psychological Research. Now you should have a good understanding of how to design and perform the experiment, as well as analyze results and apply the phenomenon.

Thanks for watching! 

Tags

Cite This
JoVE Science Education Database. Education. Ethics in Psychology Research. JoVE, Cambridge, MA, (2023).