JoVE 과학 교육
Social Psychology
JoVE 비디오를 활용하시려면 도서관을 통한 기관 구독이 필요합니다.  전체 비디오를 보시려면 로그인하거나 무료 트라이얼을 시작하세요.
JoVE 과학 교육 Social Psychology
Marginal Dishonesty: The Adding-to-10 Task
  • 00:00개요
  • 01:25Experimental Design
  • 04:04Running the Experiment
  • 05:53Representative Results
  • 07:03Applications
  • 08:40Summary

Disonestà marginale: "The adding-to-10 task"

English

소셜에 공유하기

개요

Fonte: Julian Wills & Jay Van Bavel—New York University

La teoria economica classica afferma che le persone sono razionali e interessate a se stessi. Oltre a cercare ricchezza e status, le persone sono motivate da altri obiettivi. Di conseguenza, i motivi finanziari a volte possono essere sminuiti da altre esigenze interne, come il mantenimento di un concetto di sé positivo o l’affiliazione con altri membri del gruppo.

Dilemmi etici, come la tentazione di imbrogliare sulle tasse, possono derivare quando questi motivi sono in conflitto. Da un lato, le persone possono essere tentate di risparmiare denaro sottostimando il loro reddito imponibile. D’altra parte, nessuno vuole percepirsi come un disonesto, un free-rider. Di conseguenza, le persone sono riluttanti a sfruttare appieno le opportunità non etiche perché così facendo possono minare gravemente la loro immagine di sé come individui moralmente onesti. Invece, le persone imbrogliano in misura molto minore di quanto siano in grado di fare: quanto basta per ottenere risorse aggiuntive, ma non così tanto da compromettere la loro immagine di sé.

Questa tendenza alla disonestà marginale, o il “fattore fudge”, è un principio importante nella psicologia sociale e può essere testata attraverso una varietà di tecniche. Mazar, Amir e Ariely originariamente descrissero sei esperimenti separati che coinvolgevano la (dis)onestà e una teoria del mantenimento del concetto di sé. 1 Il “Compito di aggiunta a 10” è una delle tecniche sperimentali discusse ed è prevalente nella ricerca che comporta la verifica dell’onestà. In questo video viene illustrato come produrre e interpretare l’attività Aggiunta a 10.

Principles

Procedure

1. Reclutamento dei partecipanti Condurre un’analisi di potenza e reclutare un numero sufficiente di partecipanti. Assegnare in modo casuale metà dei partecipanti alla condizione sperimentale e l’altra metà alla condizione di controllo. 2. Raccolta dei dati Offri ai partecipanti un libretto di prova con venti matrici dell’attività Adding-to-10. Ogni matrice si basa su un insieme di dodici numeri a tre cifre, due dei quali sommano esattamente a 10 …

Results

This procedure typically results in a considerably higher number of correctly "solved" questions in the experimental condition (Figure 2). This procedure can also dissociate whether this inflated performance is a result of a few individuals cheating a lot or most individuals cheating a little bit. If the former were true, this would result in a mostly overlapping distribution except for a large relative increase of individuals reporting the highest possible score. Instead, typical results reveal that most participants cheat a little bit.

Figure 1
Figure 2: A typical frequency distribution resulting from the task. In this example, there is one experimental condition and one control condition with no opportunity to cheat. The y-axis values reflect the proportion of individuals who reported correctly solving a specific number of test questions. Values on the x-axis represent bins of three numbers centered on the label displayed (e.g., 30 = participants who solved 29, 30, or 31 questions).

Applications and Summary

People inherently are torn between achieving gains from cheating versus maintaining a positive self-concept of honesty. By using techniques like the Adding-to-10 Task, modern psychological research concludes that often people, who think highly of themselves in terms of honesty, will rationalize their behavior in such a way to allow them to engage in limited dishonesty while maintaining positive views of themselves. Put another way, there is an acceptable level of dishonesty that is defined by internal reward considerations. Given these factors, dishonesty may actually decrease as external rewards increase, i.e., the internal punishment does not kick in until a certain level of gain is achieved.

Economists estimate that dishonest behaviors (e.g., cheating on tax returns, returning clothing after use, employee theft, etc.) cost organizations billions of dollars each and every year. Legislative regulations that penalize dishonesty can be expensive and exploited. In contrast, research suggests that interventions that appeal to our motives for self-image maintenance may be cheaper and more effective. For instance, research suggests that subtly priming people's self-awareness (e.g., placing a mirror behind a jar of money) can reduce theft.2

These findings also cohere with one of the core tenets of social psychology: Almost everyone is capable of misbehaving depending on the situation. Efforts to discourage cheating might be more effective if they focus less on the rare master-mind criminal and instead address the possibility that most people cheat slightly. Interventions that draw attention to ordinary people's self-image may be fruitful for reducing this temptation. For instance, Mazar et al. found that priming participants with The Ten Commandments dramatically reduced cheating (even among atheists).

References

  1. Mazar, N., Amir, O., & Ariely, D. (2008). The dishonesty of honest people: A theory of self-concept maintenance. Journal of Marketing Research, 45, 633-644.
  2. Ariely, D. (2012). The (honest) truth about dishonesty: How we lie to everyone-especially ourselves. HarpersCollins. New York.

내레이션 대본

People inherently want to reap the benefits of cheating, even if they view themselves as honest.

For example, a salesperson making a commission on every car they sell might be tempted to cheat, reporting that they sold more vehicles than they actually did. On one hand, they’ll consider the costs of this action: whether they’ll be caught and punished by an employer.

However, external rewards—like how much extra money could be made—and internal rewards—whether they can still view themselves as an honest person—also influence this decision.

As a result of this interplay between costs and rewards, many individuals might opt to be marginally dishonest—noting that they sold only a few more vehicles, rather than the entire lot. This way, they still get the benefit of having some extra cash, but their moral self-image isn’t too adversely affected.

This video explores the relationship of just how much people cheat when a cash prize is rewarded as demonstrated by the Adding-to-10 task, in which two numbers that sum to 10 are identified from a given set.

In this experiment investigating honesty, participants are asked to complete math problems—the Adding-to-10 task—and depending on their group assignment, their answers are either graded by the researcher, in the case of the controls, or themselves—the experimental condition.

The math puzzles consist of 3 x 4 matrices containing numbers below 10. The trick is that only two of the values in a grid add to 10, and—to answer the problem—each component of this adds-to-10 pair must be circled.

Participants are given a booklet of 50 such matrices to solve as many as they can within 4 min, and told that random winners will receive $10 for every correct answer. Importantly, this potential cash prize is an external reward, which encourages dishonesty at a later stage.

For the control group, workbooks are collected and graded by the researcher immediately after time is up; there is no opportunity to inflate one’s results. In contrast, participants in the experimental condition correct their own work by listening to answers the researcher reads aloud.

Afterwards, they’re told to write down how many matrices they solved on a single piece of paper. It’s emphasized that only this sheet will be collected; the rest of the workbook can be thrown away.

The idea is that, since the accuracy of self-reported grades won’t be checked, there’s no cost to cheating. Thus, participants will be tempted to lie and claim that they solved more matrices than they actually did, all in the hopes that they’ll win a larger cash prize.

Here, the dependent variable for the experimental group is the number of matrices participants report they correctly answered. This can be compared to the number of matrices that were actually solved by control participants, which serves as a baseline value.

Based on previous research, it is expected that most experimental participants will inflate their results slightly, indicating they answered several more matrices than control individuals.

Such marginal dishonesty suggests that the internal reward of still being able to view oneself as a moral person prevents most people from lying excessively, and claiming that they solved all problems.

First, to calculate how many participants are needed, perform a power analysis. To begin, greet each one when they arrive. Then, provide them with a booklet of matrices, and explain the task.

Emphasize that to correctly answer a matrix, the two values in it that add to 10 must be identified and clearly circled. Also stress that a cash prize—$10 for every correct answer—will be awarded to two random winners once all of the data are collected.

Ensure that the participant understands the task, and then allow them to solve as many math problems as possible in 4 min.

When the time is up, have the participant put their pen down. For those in the control group, immediately collect their workbook. Check each of the matrices, and note how many were answered correctly.

For members of the experimental group, explain that they will be correcting their own work as matrix answers are read aloud. Then, proceed to recite the solutions.

Emphasize that the rest of the workbook won’t be collected, and should be thrown away when they leave. Then, tell the participant to write down their name and both the number of math problems they answered correctly and incorrectly. Afterwards, have them turn in this summary answer sheet.

Once the data have been collected, debrief each individual, and explain that this task is meant to investigate how the costs and benefits of cheating influence a person’s decision to be dishonest.

To analyze the data, calculate the number of correct answers and plot as a frequency distribution showing the percentage of participants by group.

Notice that individuals allowed to self-report their results indicated that they solved a significantly higher number of matrices compared to controls.

However, based on the overlap of the distributions, most participants in the experimental condition cheated just a little with marginal inflation, and only a small percentage cheated a lot.

This suggests that participants weigh two things in their decision to be dishonest: The first is the external reward of $10 per correct matrix, which results in their lying about the number of problems they solved.

The second is the internal reward of their moral self-image—still being able to view oneself as an honest person—that effectively caps this lie, and deters participants from claiming they solved the maximum number of problems.

Now that you’ve learned how the Adding-to-10 task can be used to explore the relationship between the extent of cheating and self-image, let’s take a look at other ways researchers are investigating dishonesty.

On one hand, some scientists are evaluating how moral priming—reminding an individual about honest concepts and actions—influences their willingness to cheat. Students were asked to sign an honor code contract, and afterwards were administered the Adding-to-10 task.

It was found that there was no significant difference between the number of matrices these experimental group participants noted they solved and data from control individuals.

This suggests that prompting individuals with their institution’s honor code reminds them to be moral, and decreases their tendency to cheat.

In addition, such reminders of honesty are being extended in other settings, like an office, where supplies often go missing. In this case, it’s been shown that if a mirror is placed in an area where individuals might be tempted to steal—such as in the supply closet—they are discouraged from taking home all the pens.

The idea is that, by observing their actions, the potential thief is made self-aware, and they realize that taking all of them might mean they’re not as honest as they’d like to believe.

You’ve just watched JoVE’s video on how the Adding-to-10 task can be used to better understand dishonesty. By now, you should know how to design the experiment, collect control and cheating data, interpret the results, and grasp how self-image—especially reminding an individual of their actions—could be used to discourage cheating in other applications.

Thanks for watching!

Tags

Cite This
JoVE Science Education Database. JoVE Science Education. Marginal Dishonesty: The Adding-to-10 Task. JoVE, Cambridge, MA, (2023).