JoVE Science Education
Developmental Psychology
A subscription to JoVE is required to view this content.  Sign in or start your free trial.
JoVE Science Education Developmental Psychology
Measuring Children’s Trust in Testimony
  • 00:00Overview
  • 01:05Experimental Design
  • 02:49Running the Experiment
  • 04:47Representative Results
  • 05:52Applications
  • 06:39Summary

증언에 대한 어린이의 신뢰 측정

English

Share

Overview

출처: 주디스 다노비치와 니콜라우스 놀스 연구소 — 루이빌 대학교

사람은 주변 세상에 대해 어떻게 배우는가? 한 가지 방법은 직접 관찰과 탐사를 통해서입니다. 그러나 모든 정보를 직접 관찰할 수 있는 것은 아닙니다. 대신, 사람은 종종 정보 소스로 다른 사람에 의존해야합니다. 이는 주변 세계에 대해 많은 질문을 하지만 답변에 접근하는 수단이 제한되어 있는 어린이에게는 특히 그렇습니다. 따라서, 아이들은 자신의 질문에 대한 답변을 제공하기 위해 다른 사람에 의존해야합니다.

아이들이 속는 것을 믿고 그들이 듣는 모든 것을 믿는다는 인기있는 견해가 있습니다. 그러나, 최근 연구는이 사실이 아니라는 것을 보여주었습니다. 3세 미만의 어린이들은 다른 사람들이 말하는 것을 평가하고 다른 사람들의 간증에 선택적 신뢰를 표시한다. 아이들은 개인의 이전 행동과 특성에 대한 지식을 사용하고 해당 개인이 신뢰할 수있는 정보 소스인지 여부를 판단합니다.

이 동영상은 자작나무, 보티에, 블룸1, 코닉, 클레멘트, 해리스가 개발한 방법을 바탕으로 증언에 대한 아이들의 신뢰를 측정하는 방법을 보여줍니다. 2 

Procedure

정상적인 시력과 청각을 가진 3 세와 4 세 어린이를 모집하십시오. 이 데모를 위해 한 명의 어린이만 테스트됩니다. 실험을 수행할 때 더 큰 샘플 크기를 권장합니다. 1. 필요한 재료를 수집합니다. 서로 구별할 수 있는 두 개의 손 인형을 구하십시오. 이 데모에서는 얼굴 특징과 의류가 다른 남성 인형 두 개를 사용하십시오. 어린 아이들에게 친숙한 네 가지 공?…

Results

Researchers tested 20 3-year-old and 20 4-year-old children and found that children showed greater trust in the accurate puppet. Children were 100% accurate naming the familiar objects in the confirmation phase, suggesting they were capable of recognizing which puppet had been accurate in the history phase. The researchers found that children in both age groups chose the objects labeled by the accurate puppet at rates significantly higher than chance (75% of the time for 3-year-olds and 70% of the time for 4-year-olds; Figure 1). There were also no differences between 3- and 4-year-olds, suggesting that children in both age groups could use their observations of the puppet’s prior accuracy to make judgments about which puppet was reliable, even when the puppets were naming unfamiliar objects. 

Figure 1
Figure 1: Mean percentage of trials where children chose the object labeled by the individual who was previously accurate at labeling familiar objects.

Applications and Summary

The finding that children as young as age 3 show selective trust in information sources has important implications for how children learn about a wide range of topics. For example, when learning about the concepts underlying scientific fields, such as chemistry and biology, children typically cannot observe facts, like “There is oxygen in the air” or “Living things contain DNA,” themselves. Instead, they must rely on the testimony of other people, such as parents and teachers, and determine whether the information they receive is likely to be accurate. The same is true for learning concepts related to history (e.g., George Washington was the first president) or religion (e.g., God created the earth). The research on children’s trust suggests that, on the one hand, children as young as 3-years-old are capable of learning from more knowledgeable individuals, yet on the other hand, they keep track of how accurate the individual providing the information is likely to be and do not believe everything they hear.

Research has found that young children are also capable of making judgments about where to seek out information about different topics. They are more likely to direct questions to a previously knowledgeable individual3, and they understand that some people are experts on certain topics but not others.4 Children can think critically about information sources and where to find the answers to their questions, and they have a grasp of how knowledge is organized in other people’s minds well before they begin their formal education. Educators and parents can capitalize on children’s intuitive understanding of knowledge and expertise by providing consistently accurate information. They can also help further children’s understanding by talking to them about what makes information trustworthy or not. 

References

  1. Birch, S. A., Vauthier, S. A., & Bloom, P. Three- and four-year-olds spontaneously use others’ past performance to guide their learning. Cognition. 107, 1018–1034 (2008).
  2. Koenig, M. A., Clément, F., & Harris, P. L. Trust in testimony: Children’s use of true and false statements. Psychological Science. 15, 694–698 (2004).
  3. Koenig, M. A., & Harris, P. L. Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development. 76 (6), 1261-1277 (2005).
  4. Lutz, D. J., & Keil, F. C. Early understanding of the division of cognitive labor. Child Development. 73 (4), 1073-1084 (2002).

Transcript

Children are notorious for asking questions about the world around them, but with limited means of accessing the answers, they must rely on others to answer their questions.

On one hand, people often think that children believe everything they hear is true—like when they are told to brush their teeth every night or else, their teeth will fall out like their grandfather’s did.

However, recent research has shown that young children are not always so gullible. Rather, they evaluate an individual’s prior behavior to judge whether they are a trustworthy source of information.

Using previously developed methods, this video demonstrates a simple approach for how to design and conduct an experiment measuring young children’s trust, as well as how to analyze and interpret results regarding choices made based on conflicting sources of information.

In this experiment, young children—ages 3 to 4—are asked to label objects based on conflicting information from two different characters, in this case, hand puppets.

The two, male puppets with different facial features and clothing are introduced to children, as Ben and Tom.

During the first phase of the task, the history phase, one puppet labels four common objects accurately, whereas the other labels them all inaccurately.

Next, in the test phase, children are shown three sets of two novel and unusual objects, followed by conflicting information from the puppets about the objects’ names.

For example, Ben looks at the first item and calls it a blicket, while Tom looks at the second one and also calls it a blicket. The child is then asked to choose which item is the blicket. Note that for the remaining two sets of objects, two different words—truly and modi—are used by Ben and Tom.

Finally, in the confirmation phase, each child is asked to label the original common objects that were presented during the history phase to confirm that they were familiar with all of the items.

Here, the dependent variable is the number of times children choose the object that the accurate puppet named during three different trials in the test phase.

Three- and four-year-old children are expected to choose the objects labelled by the accurate puppet more often than the inaccurate puppet.

Before the experiment begins, obtain two physically distinct, male hand puppets, as well as four common objects: a toy car, spoon, ball, and cup. Also, obtain six novel and unusual household items, such as parts of a toy, bag clips, or felt floor pads.

After greeting the child, begin by introducing them to each puppet in first person. “Hi, I’m Ben. What’s your name?”; “Hi, I’m Tom. What’s your name?”

During the first history phase, set the four common objects on the table in front of the child. Counterbalance which puppet names the objects accurately and inaccurately across children; here, first have Ben label each of the objects correctly, and Tom label them incorrectly.

For each trial of the test phase, place a pair of novel objects on the table, counterbalanced across positions. Pick up one of the objects, and ask Ben what it is. In this trial, have him label it a blicket, “I think that is a blicket. Yes, that is a blicket” and place the object back on the table.

Then, pick up the other novel object and ask Tom what it is. Have Tom also label it a blicket. “I think that is a blicket. Yes, that is a blicket.”

After removing the puppets, close your eyes, hold out both hands, and ask the child to give you the blicket. Note the child’s choice.

Finally, for the confirmation phase, place the four common objects back on the table in front of the child. Ask the child to label each object and record their responses.

Once the study is finished, calculate the number of correct responses from the confirmation phase. To proceed with data analysis, only include children who performed well in the final naming phase to ensure that they recognized the objects and that the puppets differed in accuracy.

For each child, assign a score of 0-3, based on how many times they chose the object that the accurate puppet named in the test phase. Convert these scores to percentages.

To analyze the data, perform t-tests to determine if any differences exist between age groups or against chance levels.

Note that there were no differences between age groups. Furthermore, each group chose the objects labeled by the accurate puppet at rates significantly higher than chance levels, suggesting that even young children can use observations of prior accuracy to make judgements about trustworthiness, even when unfamiliar objects were named.

Now that you are familiar with designing a psychology experiment to demonstrate that children as young as 3 show selective trust in information sources, let’s look at how children learn about a wide range of topics from others.

In fields such as science, there are many facts that children cannot observe directly, like: There is oxygen in the air. Therefore, they must rely on the testimony of teachers and other adults to learn new information.

In addition, when children are in search of information on different topics, like what is DNA?, they are more likely to direct questions to an adult who had been previously knowledgeable. They also understand that some people are experts on certain topics, but not others.

You’ve just watched JoVE’s introduction to measuring children’s trust. Now you should have a good understanding of how to design and conduct the experiment, and finally how to analyze and interpret the results.

Thanks for watching!

Tags

Cite This
JoVE Science Education Database. JoVE Science Education. Measuring Children’s Trust in Testimony. JoVE, Cambridge, MA, (2023).