JoVE Science Education
Cognitive Psychology
A subscription to JoVE is required to view this content.  Sign in or start your free trial.
JoVE Science Education Cognitive Psychology
The Precision of Visual Working Memory with Delayed Estimation
  • 00:00Overview
  • 01:14Experimental Design
  • 03:06Running the Experiment
  • 04:17Representative Results
  • 05:35Applications
  • 06:37Summary

视觉工作记忆与延迟估计的精度

English

Share

Overview

资料来源: 实验室的乔纳森 Flombaum — — 约翰 · 霍普金斯大学

人类的记忆是有限的。纵观其历史,实验心理学有重点查处离散、 定量内存限制的 — — 一个人能记得多少单独的信息片段。最近,实验心理学家也成为兴趣更多的定性限制 — — 如何精确地存储信息?

内存精度的概念可以一次直观和难以捉摸。它是直观的例如,想一个人可以记住他们的母亲如何精确地听起来,使它能够通过电话或在人群中认出自己的母亲。但一个如何可以量化的精度,这种内存吗?到底如何类似是内存本身的声音?

学习记忆和工作记忆的精度,特别是实验心理学家发明了称为延迟估计范式。它已被用于最经常的是,到目前为止,研究视觉记忆的精度,尤其是记忆的颜色,并了解如何记忆消退的越多你设法记住一次。该视频演示了调查工作内存使用的颜色的精度标准程序延迟估计,重点放在内存如何影响作为一个试图同时记住更多的对象的颜色。

Procedure

1.刺激设计。 选择颜色的颜色,工作记忆实验,对实验的成功至关重要。它是重要的是选择驻留在相同的心理颜色圆形,所以都有相同的亮度,通过驻留在同一平面和同样的对比,身为等距的背景色的颜色的颜色。身体上,可感知的颜色被涉及一种线性尺寸,从表面反射光的波长。但是,感知,颜色空间 — — 中颜色精神如何体现的关系 — — 非直线。即使在最早的年龄,孩?…

Results

The raw data on each trial are a response color and the true target color. That means the accuracy of a response on each trial can be quantified in terms of the angular difference between the right answer and the given answer. The colors—including the target and any response—make up a ring, occupying a total of 360°. When the answer given is exactly right, the angular error is zero, and the most it can ever be is 358°.

Because the colors all have equal luminance and contrast, responses can be collapsed across color targets—analyzed just like they would be in the hypothetical case of a single color repeated many times (Figure 4). The participant does not always respond with exactly that color. They also won’t respond with ones that are very different, so the expectation is that the exactly correct color is selected most frequently. Colors very similar to it should be selected less often, but still frequently. And very different colors should be selected almost never. This kind of pattern can be described mathematically in terms of a normal distribution—a bell curve. The correct answer should be the average response over many trials, but owing to imprecision in memory, there should also be some spread—quantifying the spread amounts to quantifying the precision of color working memory (Figure 4).

Figure 4
Figure 4. Hypothetical analysis assuming the same color feature on each trial. Supposing the blue labeled “actual target color” were the memory target over many trials, one would expect that color to obtain the most responses (i.e., the highest response frequency), with roughly a normal distribution for nearby blues. In other words, one should expect normally distributed responses with the actual target as the mean of the distribution. The variability in the distribution can supply a measure of memory precision.

In other words, angular error amounts to a color-agnostic way of characterizing the accuracy of each response.

The standard deviation of the distribution is the most often used measure to describe the spread of responses. A large standard deviation means that the distribution has a lot of spread and variability, a reflection of relatively imprecise responses. A small standard deviation reflects a tight distribution and precise memory. In this way, large numbers reflect imprecision and small numbers reflect precision, so scientists often use the inverse of the standard deviation (one divided by the standard deviation) to quantify precision. Now, large numbers designate precise memories, and small numbers designate imprecise memories.

When this experiment is conducted, researchers typically compute memory precision just as described, but independently for memory load, which refers to the number of color squares in a trial.

With higher memory loads, precision tends to decline (Figure 5), suggesting a tradeoff between how many things a person can store in memory and how precisely they can store those things.

Figure 5
Figure 5. Memory precision as a function of memory load, the number of color samples to remember in a given trial. Note that the unit of precision is inverse degrees, (1/°), since the unit of angular response error and its standard deviation is degrees.

Applications and Summary

Delayed estimation is a relatively new paradigm in experimental psychology, though it has become rapidly influential. In addition to investigating tradeoffs between memory capacity and precision, it can be used to compare the precision of memory systems, such as color working memory compared to color long term memory, and also to compare precision across individuals. For example, do interior decorators or painters tend to have more precise memory of color than lawyers or doctors?

Transcript

Experimental psychologists use the delayed estimation paradigm to assess the precision of visual memories, and how such memories degrade the more one tries to remember at once.

On one hand, human memory is constrained by the number of pieces of information an individual can remember-like how many items they need to restock the pantry-meaning that it is quantitatively limited.

Memory can also be limited in its precision. For example, a person can recognize their mother on the phone because they remember the sound of her voice. However, an individual’s “stored” memory of their mother’s voice may not perfectly match its actual, physical sound. Thus, memory can also be qualitatively limited.

The delayed estimation paradigm provides a way to evaluate the relationship between these quantitative and qualitative limits of memory.

This video demonstrates methods for investigating the precision of visual working memory, including how to design the stimulus and perform an experiment involving a delayed estimation paradigm, as well as how to analyze and interpret the results.

In this experiment, color provides an ideal stimulus to evaluate the precision of visual working memory, as it can be mentally represented in a continuous, non-linear spectrum known as a color ring.

Participants are asked to perform several trials during which they must remember a color stimulus. Each of these trials consists of three phases: sample, delay, and test.

During the sample phase, a randomly colored square appears onscreen for 500 ms. The square then disappears, leaving a blank screen.

Through this delay phase, participants are asked to focus on the empty screen for 900 ms, whereby the sample color must be remembered.

In the final test phase, a probe square-outlined in black and devoid of color-appears in the same position as the colored box shown previously.

Simultaneously, participants are shown a color ring consisting of 180 different colors and asked to select the region of the color ring that most closely resembles the original sample color.

Note that the color ring always appears in a random orientation, which ensures that participants can’t associate specific areas onscreen with certain colors.

To increase task difficulty, the memory load-the number of colored boxes shown in each trial-is varied from one to eight.

The dependent variable then is the precision of color working memory-how accurately participants remember the color or colors shown during the sample phase.

For a given sample color, participants are expected to vary within the “true” color range, but rarely choose colors that are drastically different.

As memory load increases, the precision of color working memory is likely to decrease.

To begin, choose a set of 180 colors with varying hues, which together form a color ring. Check that these colors demonstrate the same light intensity and contrast relative to the background color on the screen; this ensures that no single color will be more memorable to participants during the trials.

When the participant arrives, direct them to a computer and explain the procedure of the experiment.

Emphasize that when a specific region of the screen is probed, only the color of the box that appeared earlier in that same position should be chosen. In addition, instruct the participant to guess if they are unsure of a probed sample color.

To assure that participants understand the task, allow them to perform ten practice trials.

Once the participant understands the instructions, have them complete 480 experimental trials, with equal numbers of trials for memory loads between one and eight.

For each trial, record the memory load, the true colors of the sample boxes, and the colors the participant chose after the delay period.

To analyze the data independent of color, for every sample box shown and probed in a trial, calculate the angular error-the distance in degrees between the true and chosen response colors on the color ring.

If the participant remembered the exact color of the sample box after the delay period, the angular error should be zero.

For each group of trials dealing with the same memory load, create a frequency distribution curve, where the angular error is plotted on the X axis and frequency on the Y axis.

Once frequency distribution curves have been generated, calculate the standard deviation-the spread of values around the mean-for each.

Take the inverse of the standard deviation to generate a value representing memory precision. If this value is large, this is indicative of memory being precise for a group of trials.

To visualize the data, plot the calculated memory precision values as a function of memory load. Notice that as load increases, memory precision tends to decline, suggesting a tradeoff between how many things a participant can remember at once, and how precisely they can store this information.

Now that you know how to design and perform an experiment using delayed estimation, let’s look at how researchers are currently using this paradigm to tease apart different aspects of visual memory.

Until now, we’ve discussed how delayed estimation has been used to assess short-term working memory, in which a participant only has to briefly store a piece of color information for a single a trial. However, researchers could also investigate long-term color memory with this paradigm, evaluating it over much longer periods.

Furthermore, this paradigm can also be used to compare the precision of visual memory between different individuals, for example, visual-based professionals like interior designers, and potentially less-visual subjects, such as lawyers or doctors.

Finally, although researchers typically use the delayed estimation paradigm to evaluate memory for color, it can also be employed in neurocognitive assessments of other types of visual working memory-like that pertaining to shapes.

You’ve just watched JoVE’s introduction to delayed estimation. We’ve reviewed how to perform this method, as well as collect and analyze participant color memory data. Importantly, we’ve noted how this technique can help understand how the qualitative limits of human color memory can be influenced by quantitative factors.

Thanks for watching!

Tags

Cite This
JoVE Science Education Database. JoVE Science Education. The Precision of Visual Working Memory with Delayed Estimation. JoVE, Cambridge, MA, (2023).