JoVE Science Education
Neuropsychology
This content is Free Access.
JoVE Science Education Neuropsychology
The Split Brain
  • 00:00Overview
  • 01:55Experimental Design
  • 05:34Running the Experiment
  • 07:34Representative Results
  • 08:32Applications
  • 10:00Summary

スプリット ブレイン

English

Share

Overview

ソース: ジョナス ・ t. カプラン サラ I. ギンベル所-南カリフォルニア大学

脳の損傷により認知機能がどのような影響を与える研究認知神経科学の最も重要なツールの一つとされてきました。脳は体の最もよく保護された部分の 1 つで、脳の機能に影響を与えることができる多くのイベントがあります。血管の問題、腫瘍、変性疾患、感染症、鈍力外傷、脳神経外科は脳損傷、脳のさまざまな方法で機能に影響を与える組織の損傷のさまざまなパターンを生成可能性がありますすべての根本的な原因のほんの一部です。

神経心理学の歴史は、脳の理解の進歩につながったいくつかのよく知られている例で示されます。例えばで 1861 ポール ブローカー様子が観察された失語症、獲得した言語障害で起因した前頭葉左への損傷。別の例として多大なメモリについてきた神経心理学の文献の多くの年のため「陛下、「特定種類の新しい記憶の形成に深刻な赤字につながった-が側頭葉手術として知られているヘンリー Molaison の有名な事件など、記憶喪失の患者から。

観察および焦点の脳損傷患者のテスト中には、細心の注意、脳の機能に洞察力と神経科学は、赤字の特定の性質を明らかにするテストの設計で取られなければならない提供をしています。また、脳が相互接続されたニューロンの複雑なネットワークには、1 つの脳の領域への損傷は損傷から遠く離れた地域で機能している影響ことができます。脳の損傷が脳の領域間の接続に与える影響を示すため、このビデオは、いわゆるスプリット ブレインの場合を調べます。

脳の左右の半球を接続する線維の大きな束である脳梁脳の最大の白質路の一つであり脳の正中線の矢状ビューに簡単に認識することができます。1960 年代、脳神経外科医は、脳梁を切断はできる特定の種類のてんかんは、脳に広がる手に負えないの神経活動を含む治療の成功を発見しました。ブレインの手術を受けた人に分離手術の 2 つの半球が持っていたは、左と右半球が通信することが不要になったようなもの。この条件は、左と右の脳半球の機能を相対的な能力およびそれらの間の通信の性質について学習する独立して、プローブに実験者を許可しました。

このビデオは、スプリットブレイン患者脳の 2 つの半球間の相違のいくつかを明らかにするため、このような切断のいくつかの劇的な結果を参照してくださいにテストする方法を示します。これらの実験の元のバージョンはマイケル ・ ガッザニガと同僚1、2によって開発され、後に他人によって練られました。3バージョンがここには、方法論の最近の近代化が組み込まれています。

Procedure

1. 患者とコントロールの募集 完全および部分的な手術 callosotomies と、脳梁が完全に発達していなかった脳梁 (ACC) 欠損症などの先天性の条件を含む切断症候群患者のさまざまながあります。2 つの半球を接続する複数の管があります。最大の脳梁は、しかし、いくつかの繊維は前交連、海馬交連、後交連で交差します。 切断のこれらの異なる品種がこのテストの別の行動の結果につながる可能性がありますに注意してください。 この実験のため接続ファイバーの有無を確認する神経イメージング研究を使用して患者を事前に選択します。 標準的な MRI や画像白質路に使用できます、拡散イメージングは、特に便利です。接続する繊維が患者の存在を知ることは、結果の解釈に役立ちます。このデモでは、完全な脳梁離断症例が選択されています。 患者研究手順の完全に通知されている、すべての適切な同意書を締結したことを確認します。 同じ年齢や性別、患者としての 20 の参加者を募集する知能、ウェクスラー成人知能スケール (WAIS) のスコアを使用して一致します。 2. データの収集 だけ左または右半球に視覚刺激を提示するために、1 つの視覚野に刺激を適切に提示する必要があります。これが 1 つ目に刺激と同等ではないことに注意してください。それぞれの目は、脳の両半球にプロジェクトします。たとえば、左の視野を見る左目の部分は右半球によって処理されますが、左半球で処理右視野の左目の部分を見ています。したがって、左半球にイメージを提示する患者が見ている場所の右側にある右視野内に完全紹介します。 この左右差を達成するため、コンピューターの画面からおよそ 22 インチ目を維持するために、あごを使用します。画面に直面している、あご内快適患者の顎を配置します。 患者が彼らの目を凝視するための場所を提供するために画面の中央に残っている小さな十字があります。 左またはそれの右側に画像が表示されるとしても、実験を通してこのクロスの固定を維持するために患者に指示します。 画像が表示されたら、オブジェクトの名前を声に出して言う必要があります彼らは患者へ説明します。 脳の左右の半球にそれぞれ投影画面の左右に簡単によく知られている物体の画像を提示します。50 個のイメージは、リンゴ、ボール、ほうき、鶏など、わかりやすい図面を含むオブジェクトの集合からランダムな順序で掲載されています。 適切な左右差を確保するため 150 ミリ秒未満の画像を提示します。これは、刺激を見る十分な時間が、十分高速なので患者が中心視野に刺激を参照してくださいに目を移動することができるではないです。 大声で、画面に表示されるオブジェクトの名前に患者をお願いし、その応答を記録します。これは口頭言語能力のテスト、スピーキング能力の半球間の相違点を明らかにする必要があります。 せれば (上と同じ側) と同側の手で、紙を見もせず、オブジェクトを描画する患者の患者は任意のオブジェクトに名前を付けることができる、刺激。これは刺激の知識の非言語的手段として提供しています。 刺激と同側の手は、刺激を見た半球によって制御されます。たとえば、左視野の刺激策を発表すると、それは右半球によって処理されます。右半球は左の手の制御を主担当です。 それが 1 つの半球に刺激の分離を維持するために描画中に患者は自分の手を見ていないことを確認します。 患者は、オブジェクトの描画を終了したときは、オブジェクトを見て、大声だと言ってもらいます。これは、患者を知っているとオブジェクトの名前は、中心の展望で提示彼らはそれが 1 つの半球に提示されるときに名前を付けることができない場合でも確認できます。 各コントロールの参加者に対して手順を繰り返します。 3. データ分析 患者さんのパフォーマンスを解析するには、互いの左側と右側の視覚の半分フィールドからのデータを比較します。これを行うには、各視野の正しいと不適切な回答数を集計し、独立性のカイ二乗検定を用いて観察 1 つと同じ大きさの差を得られる可能性をテストします。 年齢、性別、および欠損患者の行動を決定するための知能をマッチさせた対照人口のデータを患者からデータを比較します。これを行うにを個別に、それぞれの人の左視野と右視野の平均スコアをコンパイルおよび比較するには、繰り返し対策分析の分散の使用分布は (ANOVA) をテストします。

Results

Typically, callosotomy patients exhibit an anomia for objects presented in the left visual half-field. Anomia is the inability to name objects. Objects presented to the right visual field, however, are named with high accuracy (Figure 1).

Figure 1
Figure 1: Patient and control performance in the naming objects task for stimuli presented in the left and right visual fields. The patient (black circles) is not able to verbally name objects presented in the left visual field, but is able to name objects in the right visual field. In contrast, the control population (blue diamonds) can name objects presented in both the left and right visual fields.

Some patients may be able to successfully draw objects presented to the left visual field, even though they cannot verbally name them (Figure 2).

Figure 2
Figure 2: Patient and control performance in the drawing objects task for stimuli presented in the left and right visual fields. The patient (black circles) and control population (blue diamonds) are able to draw objects presented in both the left and right visual fields. The patient's performance does not differ from matched controls.

In this case, the patient usually says they haven't seen anything. This is because the left hemisphere, which is controlling speech, has not seen the visual image. However, the right hemisphere, which has seen the object, can recognize it but is unable to generate speech. Since the right hemisphere is largely in control of the left hand, the patient is able to draw the object with the left hand. This result demonstrates a dissociation between the ability to recognize an object and the ability to verbally name an object.

The control population, with intact corpora callosa, can both name and draw objects presented in the left or right visual fields. This is because information can freely pass from one hemisphere to the other, allowing for the sharing of information between the brain regions.

Applications and Summary

The case of the split-brain patient reveals the relative specialization of the two cerebral hemispheres. Many of these specializations can also be demonstrated in healthy people with intact commissures using similar techniques. For example, people tend to recognize words faster when they are presented briefly in the right visual field compared to when they are presented in the left visual field. This experiment also shows that even when two brain regions are healthy, damage to the connections between different regions can affect behavior.

However, it is important to remember that while testing the split brain demonstrates the differences between the two cerebral hemispheres, in the intact brain, the two hemispheres are continually interacting with each other and working in concert. To isolate a stimulus to one visual field requires specialized equipment that can present stimuli very briefly and away from central fixation. Since central vision is processed by both hemispheres, and the eyes typically scan an environment, this is not a situation that is likely to be encountered in everyday life.

References

  1. Gazzaniga, M. S., Bogen, J. E., & Sperry, R. W. (1962). Some functional effects of sectioning the cerebral commissures in man. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 48, 1765-1769.
  2. Gazzaniga, M. S., Bogen, J. E., & Sperry, R. W. (1965). Observations on visual perception after disconnexion of the cerebral hemispheres in man. Brain, 88(2), 221-236.
  3. Zaidel, E., Zaidel, D., & Bogen, J. E. (1990). Testing the commussurotomy patient. In A. Boulton, G. Baker, & M. Hiscock (Eds.), Neuromethods (pp. 147-201). Clifton, NJ: Humana Press.

Transcript

Neuropsychologists study “split-brain” patients to probe the unique functions of the left and right brain hemispheres—in other words, to study lateralization—and to also investigate the nature of communication between these regions.

Primarily speaking, information from one side of the body is processed within the opposite half of the brain. In addition, each hemisphere contralaterally directs body movements.

These areas also have different cognitive strengths: the left side is typically associated with the control of language and speech, whereas the right plays a large role in processing visuospatial information—like judging the spatial arrangements of dials on a machine.

Normally, collections of neurons’ axons—referred to as nerve fiber bundles—transfer information between these hemispheres. One of the largest of such tracts is the corpus callosum.

However, this inter-hemispheric communication is interrupted in split-brain patients, whose corpora callosa have been surgically severed—a treatment sometimes used to reduce the uncontrollable neural activity characteristic of epilepsy from spreading throughout the entire brain.

Using modernizations of psychologist Michael Gazzaniga’s techniques, this video demonstrates how to test split-brain patients and assess their cognitive abilities—specifically speech production—and illustrates data collection and analysis methods.

In this experiment, patients are shown images of everyday objects and asked to verbalize the name of each item.

To achieve lateralization, patients are instructed to focus on a cross symbol in the center of a computer screen, and told to remain fixated on this shape for the duration of the experiment. Here, the cross serves as a reference point next to which visual stimuli can be shown on either the right or left.

If an image is presented on the right of the screen, it falls into the right visual field—which, perhaps counterintuitively, is processed by the left portions of both eyes. These regions then project the observed image to the left hemisphere of the brain, where it is identified.

Thus, functions of the left brain hemisphere can be assessed by showing images in the right visual field.

Similarly, a stimulus presented to the left of the cross onscreen—in the left visual field—can be used to evaluate the roles of the right hemisphere.

During the naming objects task, a total of fifty drawings, like that of a chicken, appear one at a time on a random side of the monitor—either the right or left.

Pictures are presented for less than 150 ms. As this is not enough time for a patient to move their eyes to reposition the image, it ensures that only the brain hemisphere being tested “sees” the stimulus.

After the image disappears, the patient must identify it aloud, which serves as a measure of the lateralization of verbal linguistic capability.

Here, the dependent variable is the percentage of images shown in the left and right visual fields that the patient is able to name—in other words, the accuracy of verbal identification.

Based on the previous work of Gazzaniga and others, it is expected that patients will be able to name images presented in the right visual field with high accuracy, as this information is seen by the left hemisphere—the region capable of controlling speech.

However, patients will be unable to verbally identify pictures shown in the left visual field, as this information is handled by the right brain hemisphere, which is incapable of generating language and—in split-brain patients—cannot communicate with the speech-capable left side.

If the image can’t be named—referred to as anomia—a drawing task is performed, which serves as a non-linguistic measure of stimulus knowledge.

Here, patients must create a picture of the image they were shown using the hand on the ipsilateral or same side as the tested visual field. Thus, if patients can’t verbally identify an object presented on the left of the screen, they should draw it with their left hand.

In this instance, the dependent variable is the percentage of images shown in the left and right visual fields that are accurately drawn.

It is expected that patients unable to name pictures shown on the left of the monitor will still be able to draw them—using their left hand—with high accuracy.

This is due to the fact that the right hemisphere—which controls the left arm and hand—also processes information from the left visual field. Thus, no communication is needed between the hemispheres to complete this task.

Prior to beginning the experiment, review patients’ MRI data to determine which nerve fiber bundles they are lacking. For the purpose of this demonstration, a patient in whom the entire corpus callosum has been severed is tested, and their data will be compared to those collected from control participants.

Greet the patient when they arrive, and inform them of the research procedures. In addition, assure that they have signed all appropriate consent forms.

Then, proceed to place their chin comfortably in a chinrest so that their eyes are positioned approximately 22 in. from the screen.

With the small cross displayed in the center of the screen, emphasize to the patient that they must remain fixated on this symbol, even as images flash to the left or right of it.

Proceed by showing them 50 images, each of which are presented for 150 ms, in a random order, and evenly divided between sides. After every presentation, instruct the patient to identify the object out loud: “Apple”. Record all of their responses.

If the patient cannot name the visual stimulus, ask them to draw it with the hand on the same side as the visual field in which the picture was shown. This constitutes the drawing objects task.

Make sure that the patient does not look at their hand as they are drawing, in order to maintain the initial isolation of the stimulus to one brain hemisphere.

To confirm that the patient knows the name of the stimulus when it is simultaneously presented to both fields of view, have them look down at their completed drawing and verbally identify the object it represents: “Broom”. Again, record all of the patient’s responses.

To analyze the data, first calculate the percentage of correct verbal responses across patients for stimuli presented to the left and right visual fields.

Proceed by separately compiling the percentage of correct verbal response scores for each control participant’s left and right locations.

To identify any deficits in patient behavior, compare control and patient data using a repeated-measures analysis of variance test. Repeat the analysis for all data collected from the drawing test.

Notice that while patients are typically unable to name stimuli presented to the left visual field, they can draw them—with their left hand—with a high degree of accuracy. This demonstrates a dissociation between a patient’s ability to recognize and verbally name an object.

Now that you know how to test the functions of the left and right hemispheres of split-brain patients with visual stimuli, let’s see how researchers explore and apply lateralization in other contexts.

You’ve learned that surgical separation of the two hemispheres is often used to treat patients with epilepsy, which is characterized by seizures.

As a result, many neuroscientists are looking at whether the timing of this disconnection—whether the corpus callosum is severed during childhood or adulthood—has any affect on a patient’s cognitive functions.

Importantly, such work has demonstrated that compared to adults, children experience fewer—or less severe—cognitive effects following the disconnection of the brain hemispheres, suggesting that young brains demonstrate a great degree of plasticity.

Up until now, we’ve focused on the corpus callosum as the major connection between the left and right hemispheres.

However, other nerve fiber bundles allow for communication between the sides of the brain. Among them is the anterior commissure, which has been implicated in the transfer of sensory information, like that pertaining to sight or smell.

Thus, some researchers are looking at how the disconnection of one or more of these bundles—with or without severance of the corpus callosum—affects patient behavior.

You’ve just watched JoVE’s video on testing split-brain patients using visual stimuli. By now, you should understand how to present images to the two visual fields, and collect and interpret data relating to the abilities of the left and right brain hemispheres. You should also know how data from split-brain patients is being used to develop better treatments for epilepsy, and understand the roles of different nerve fiber bundles in the brain.

Thanks for watching!

Tags

Cite This
JoVE Science Education Database. JoVE Science Education. The Split Brain. JoVE, Cambridge, MA, (2023).