JoVE Science Education
Social Psychology
This content is Free Access.
JoVE Science Education Social Psychology
The Implicit Association Test
  • 00:00Vue d'ensemble
  • 01:19Experimental Design
  • 04:32Running the Experiment
  • 06:49Representative Results
  • 08:29Applications
  • 09:45Summary

内隐联想测验

English

Diviser

Vue d'ensemble

资料来源: 朱利安遗嘱 & 杰范韦尔 — — 纽约大学

社会心理学中的核心构造之一就是态度的对一个物体或人的概念。传统上,心理学家测定态度只要求人们自我报告他们的信仰、 意见或感情。这种方法有局限性,但是,当测量社会敏感的态度,像种族偏见,因为人们往往积极地自我报告、 不带偏见的平等主义信仰 (尽管窝藏负面联想)。为了绕开这个社会理想的偏见,心理学家已经尝试测量内隐态度是不服从故意控制 (和潜在的扭曲) 的任务数目。

内隐联结测验或内隐联想测验,是最具有影响力的措施,这些无意识的态度之一。IAT 安东尼 · 格林沃尔德和他的同事首先介绍 1998 年的报纸了。1这个视频将演示如何进行 IAT 在最后的实验中,使用在欧洲美国的参加者 (报告明确的平等主义态度) 展示出隐式偏好为他们自己的种族。

Procédure

1.参与者招聘 进行了动力分析和招聘足够的参与者能够得到足够的统计能力去侦测观察到的效果大小,然后获得知情同意。 2.数据收集 组装与愉快 12 个字的列表 — — 好 — — 协会 (例如,快乐,幸运地,礼物) 和 12 个字与不愉快 — — 坏 — — 协会 (例如,仇恨,灾难,毒)。 装配 12 欧洲美国和 12 美国黑人的脸 (一半男性,一半女性),裁剪,并在下巴和额头。 与五个块 (即、 序列) 创建一个刺激演示文稿的脚本。 该脚本应该只允许三个键盘响应: (1) ‘空格键’ 推进说明,(2)’ E’ 选择锚索在左边,和 (3) 我,选择右边的锚。 除了记录参与者的按键,脚本也应记录其响应延迟 (即,每个刺激演示文稿和提供应答之间的时间)。 块 1: 初始目标概念歧视。比赛锚定一侧的屏幕和白色显示黑色的安装程序将显示在的另一侧。特定的方向应该抵消跨科目 (即,一半受试者应该看到左边的黑色和白色在右边)。此块组成的 40 项试验总: 第一次 20 是实践试验,而在后者 20 试验分析数据。随机样本 (不替换) 五十一面临以便显示每个是完全两次 (一次为实践试验,一次真正的试验)。每项试验显示分隔间审判延迟 100 毫秒、 400 毫秒,或 700 ms (随机选择每个审判)。 块 2: 关联属性歧视。设置价锚,使愉快在一侧的屏幕上显示和不愉快显示在另一侧。此块具有所有的相同特征作为块 1 (即,制衡,数量的试验,拖延间审判) 除了显示效价词 (例如,好的坏的) 而不是脸。 块 3: 初始结合任务。显示种族和价锚 (即,如果黑色显示在右侧块 1,那么它也应出现在此块的右边) 作为前块使用相同的方向。目前每个 12 的面孔和 12 个价字至少两次,共计 40 项试验。使用同一间拖延审理从前面的块。 块 4: 扭转目标概念歧视。删除价锚和交换比赛锚定,黑现在显示在屏幕上白色的最初显示位置 (反之亦然)。否则,此块保留块 1 的所有的特性。 块 5: 反向组合的任务。除了种族锚现在处于相同的位置因为他们在块 4,此块是对块 3 一样的。应该在参与者抵消块 3 和 5。 对于所有块,指导参与者应尽快分类面孔/条款基础,与之关联的类别。 完成后的计算机管理内隐联想测验任务,分发衡量有关种族态度和信仰的几个调查问卷。 若要允许参与者知道他们有隐私,让他们填完这些问卷中个人实验房间。此外通知他们,他们将放他们填妥的问卷在没有标记的信封返回之前对它们的实验者。 调查应包括一个感觉温度计和针对黑色和白色的种族概念的语义差别措施 — — 现代种族主义规模2— — 以及多样性和歧视鳞片。3 7 点鳞片每五个语义维度与以下的极性相反形容词对子用作锚: 美丽 — — 丑陋,好 — — 坏,令人愉快 — — 令人不快、 诚实 — — 不诚实,好 — — 可怕。 从使用这些语义的五个维度的所有四个对象类别对项目进行分级指导参与者。 指示参与者,如果他们认为这两种锚固的形容词是无关的类别标记中等规模。 解说: 要完成这项研究,告诉参与者的确切性质的研究。 3.数据分析 所以,这些极端的意见不会不适当地影响分析,重新编码反应时间少于 300 毫秒为 300 毫秒的反应时间长于 3000 到 3000 ms,ms。 因为积极倾斜反应时间数据,日志变换反应时间的所有数据,它更服从正态分布。 然后比较上块 3 块 5 的平均反应时间。 减去这些分数来计算 IAT 效应的一项指标。 积极分数反映出隐含的偏爱黑与白 (即,支持黑人),负分数反映隐偏好白色与黑色 (即亲白色),而评分为零指示为黑色和白色的等效隐含的偏爱。 通过平均每个概念,利用规模从-3 (负) 到 3 (积极) 的五个维度的显式评级计算语义的微分积分。

Résultats

This procedure typically results in considerably slower responses during Black/pleasant compared to White/pleasant trials (Figure 1). Since slower responses are interpreted to reflect more difficult associations, this longer relative latency (i.e., delay) suggests an implicit attitudinal preference for White over Black. That is, subjects typically find it more challenging to associate Black faces with pleasant nouns. Moreover, when exclusively analyzing responses from White participants, for instance, they often self-report egalitarian preferences (i.e., no preference for either White or Black), despite IAT scores that reveal a strong implicit preference for White over Black (Figure 2).

Figure 1
Figure 1. A typical outcome of the Implicit Association Test. White subjects who performed the Black/pleasant block first. The mean reaction time scores (untransformed) are displayed on the y-axis with error bars equal to one standard deviation. Although the reaction times are log transformed for the analysis, the untransformed scores are displayed for easier interpretation. The x-axis displays the order in which these subjects encountered these blocks. This figure was adapted from Greenwald, McGhee, and Schwartz.1

Figure 2
Figure 2. Relationship of IAT scores to explicit preferences among White participants. The IAT effects scores are displayed on the y-axis with positive scores indicating pro-Black preferences, negative scores indicating pro-White preferences, and zero indicating no differential preference. The semantic differential scores are displayed on the x-axis with positive scores indicating pro-Black preferences, negative scores indicating pro-White preferences, and zero indicating no differential preference. Virtually all White participants that report an explicit pro-Black or egalitarian (i.e., score of zero) semantic preference also show a pro-White preference on the IAT. This figure was adapted from Greenwald, McGhee, and Schwartz.1

Applications and Summary

Since the original paper, the IAT has been extended to examine prejudice in many other domains, such as gender, religion, and sexuality.4 In addition, the IAT has been adapted to (1) dissociate implicit attitudes from stereotypes, (2) measure self-esteem by pairing self/other with pleasant/unpleasant words, and (3) reveal implicit attitudes in children. In some cases, the IAT provides better predictive validity than self-report measures, such as discrimination and suicidal behavior.5

One of the reasons it has become so influential is that it had been made available online at a website called Project Implicit (https://implicit.harvard.edu/implicit/) where anyone can participate in multiple versions. Millions of people have now completed the measure and they have received immediate feedback on how their own implicit preferences compare to other people who have completed the test. The research on implicit bias has had massive implications outside the field of psychology, and implicit bias training is now common in major organizations, governmental agencies, and police departments.

References

  1. Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the Implicit Association Test. Journal of personality and social psychology, 74, 1464.
  2. McConahay, J. B., Hardee, B. B., & Batts, V. (1981). Has racism declined in America? It depends on who is asking and what is asked. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 25, 563-579.
  3. Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. ( 1997). Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 262-274.
  4. Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Hansen, J. J., Devos, T., Lindner, N. M., Ranganath, K. A., Smith, C.T., et al. (2007). Pervasiveness and correlates of implicit attitudes and stereotypes. European Review of Social Psychology, 18, 36-88.
  5. Nock, M. K., Park, J. M., Finn, C. T., Deliberto, T. L., Dour, H. J., & Banaji, M. R. (2010). Measuring the suicidal mind: implicit cognition predicts suicidal behavior. Psychological Science, 21, 511-517.

Transcription

Asking someone to say what’s on their mind can be disconnected from the beliefs that they are willing to discuss or are even knowingly aware of.

While traditional methods often ask individuals to report on their own attitudes—say to evaluate personal feelings about members of a stigmatized group—their opinion is explicit, involving deliberate thought.

Because the topic is sensitive, people are more likely to state unprejudiced, egalitarian views and portray themselves positively even though they may truly harbor negativity.

To circumvent this social-desirability bias, implicit attitudes—evaluations that occur outside of conscious awareness and control—must be examined.

This video demonstrates how to conduct the Implicit Association Test—an influential measure for investigating the strength of associations between a concept like race and automatic evaluations based on the original work of Greenwald and colleagues.

In this experiment, European American participants are recruited to establish out-group homogeneity. They are shown pictures and words—associated with either a particular race or attribute—and asked to quickly and accurately sort them across five different blocks.

The catch is that the stimuli appear in rapid succession—without time for participants to explicitly process them—hence, the name Implicit Association Test.

In the first block of trials, concept discrimination, the initial targets—faces of White European or Black African origin—are randomly presented, without replacement.

If the face shown is White, participants should press the key corresponding to “White”. The first half are considered practice trials, and not saved, as errors are expected while participants get used to responding quickly.

Likewise, for Block 2—attribute discrimination—participants are exposed to only “good” and “bad” words. That is, if “horrible” appears, the correct reply would be a keypress corresponding to “bad”. Thus, the first two blocks serve as baseline latencies associated with correct responses.

During Block 3—the combined portion—either images or words are shown with a race and attribute paired to one response key. Participants must now decide whether the presented face or word like “fabulous” corresponds with “Black or Good” or “White or Bad”.

Block 4—the reversed concept discrimination—is a repeat of Block 1 except that the computer keys for “Black” and “White” are flipped. With this block, participants adjust to the keypresses for the new “Black/White” correspondences.

Finally, Block 5—reversed combined—is similar to Block 3 except that Race is flipped across attributes, such that “Black or Bad” and “White or Good” are now paired on the same response key. Implicit preferences should be observed in latency differences compared to Block 3.

The dependent variable then is the latency to react across block types. Participants are expected to sort faster when good words and White faces go with the same key compared to the opposite, good and Black. Thus, reaction times reveal the strength of each participant’s implicit preference, which agrees with a stereotypical bias.

In addition, the responses can be log-transformed and compared to self-reported attitudes provided on a race-related questionnaire given after the task has been completed.

In this case, if participants’ biases are indeed unspoken, it’s hypothesized that the self-reported beliefs will not correlate to the scores identified during the Implicit Association Test, thus revealing a form of social-desirability.

Before starting the experiment, conduct a power analysis to determine the appropriate number of participants—specifically of European-American descent—that are required.

To begin, greet each one in the lab, explain they will be sorting images and words over several blocks of trials, and have them sign a consent form to participate.

Seat the participant in front of a computer. Further explain that they are to classify, as quickly and accurately as possible, the word or image that appears on the screen by pressing “E” if it fits the category on the left or “I” for the right side. Answer any questions and leave the room.

Start Block 1 by pressing the spacebar. During this initial phase, notice that participants are merely responding to classify faces based on the race anchors Black and White over the course of 100 trials.

Proceeding to Block 2, associated attribute discrimination, observe that now only the list of words and the valence anchors good and bad serve as the classification choices for another 100 trials.

Upon progressing into Block 3, the initial combined task, either images or words appear—but are now paired onto one response key—for a total of 200 trials.

Block 4, reversed target-concept discrimination, is related to Block 1, except that the race anchors now appear on the opposite side for 100 trials.

Finally in Block 5, the reversed combined segment, notice that participants again classify faces and words with combined anchors but the attributes are flipped compared to Block 3.

Following the Implicit Association Test, explain that there are some additional questionnaires to complete on the computer. Emphasize that they will have complete privacy, and then leave the room.

Allow participants enough time to complete the surveys. To conclude, return for debriefing and thank them for taking part in the study.

To visualize the data, plot the mean trial latencies across block type. See the manuscript for details on recoding reaction times.

Notice that responses were slower for the Black/good trials as compared to the White/good ones. Slower responses reflect more difficult associations, suggesting European-American participants found it challenging to associate Black faces with pleasant nouns. In other words, they exhibited an implicit attitudinal preference for the White anchor over the Black anchor.

Also for each participant, compute an index of the Implicit Association Effect by first log transforming the reaction times and then subtracting the means in Block 3 from Block 5. A positive score reflects an automatic preference for Black, whereas a negative one reveals an inclination for White.

Compare these indexes with the semantic differential score calculated by averaging the explicit ratings on the final questionnaire. Here, a value of zero indicates a self-reported egalitarian preference—no race bias.

Results show that most participants did self-report egalitarian preferences despite their IAT scores revealing a moderate to strong implicit preference for White over Black. These results suggest that a social-desirability bias may have distorted their responses to the questionnaire.

Now that you are familiar with how the Implicit Association Test can examine automatic bias and prejudice for socially sensitive topics, let’s look at other real-life situations where the task can be applied.

Researchers have published the Implicit Association Test on the internet allowing anyone to participate, on a number of topics, including social attitudes and mental health. Participants receive immediate feedback on their implicit preferences, providing a straight-forward approach for examining their own beliefs and biases in the comfort of their own home or office.

Other researchers have used the task to measure self-esteem by pairing self/other with pleasant/unpleasant words. These tests can see through any fa¸ade or deception leading to better success with interventions and treatment.

Lastly, children are notoriously difficult to get reliable information from, especially when asked to self-report measures. For this reason, researchers use Implicit Association Tests to assess young attitudes and beliefs, ranging from versions that investigate preferences towards race, gender, and even healthy-eating behaviors.

You’ve just watched JoVE’s video on the Implicit Association Test. Now you should have a good understanding of how to design and execute an experiment using Implicit Association tests, how to analyze and assess the results, as well as how to apply the principles to a number of real-world situations.

Thanks for watching!

Tags

Cite This
JoVE Science Education Database. Education. The Implicit Association Test. JoVE, Cambridge, MA, (2023).