JoVE Science Education
Social Psychology
This content is Free Access.
JoVE Science Education Social Psychology
The Implicit Association Test
  • 00:00Vue d'ensemble
  • 01:19Experimental Design
  • 04:32Running the Experiment
  • 06:49Representative Results
  • 08:29Applications
  • 09:45Summary

潜在連合テスト

English

Diviser

Vue d'ensemble

ソース: ジュリアン遺言 & ジェイ ・ ヴァン ・ Bavel-ニューヨーク大学

社会心理学のコア構成要素の 1 つは、オブジェクトや人に対する態度の概念です。従来、心理学者は、単に自分の信念、意見や感情を自己報告する人々 を尋ねることによって態度を測定しました。このアプローチ (否定的な連想をかくまっている) にもかかわらず公平、平等主義の信念を自己報告する独創力のある人が多いので、人種的偏見のような社会的にデリケートな態度を測定する場合、制限があります。この社会的望ましさバイアスを回避するために心理学者は、意図的なコントロール (および潜在的な歪み) により少なく適している潜在的態度を測定しようとするタスクの数を開発しています。

潜在連合テスト、または、IAT はこれらの意識不明の態度の最も有力な対策の一つです。IAT は、アンソニー ・ グリーンワルド氏と同僚によって 1998 年の論文で最初に導入されました。1このビデオは最後の実験で使用される (who は明示的な平等主義の態度を報告) ヨーロッパのアメリカ人参加者が自分のレースの暗黙的な設定を示す IAT を実施する方法を実演します。

Procédure

1. 参加者募集 パワー解析を行い、観察された効果のサイズを検出し、インフォームド・コンセントを取得するための十分な統計的検出力を得るに十分な参加者を募集します。 2. データの収集 快適な 12 の単語のリストを組み立てる-良い-団体 (例えば、ハッピー、ラッキー、ギフト) と不愉快の 12 の言葉-悪い-団体 (例えば憎しみ、災害、毒)。 12 ヨーロッパ アメリカとあごと額にトリミング 12 のアフリカ系アメリカ人の顔 (半分男性、ハーフの女性)、まとめます。 5 ブロック (すなわちシーケンス) で刺激プレゼンテーション スクリプトを作成します。 スクリプト必要がありますのみ 3 つのキーボードの応答を許可する: (1)「スペース」については、推進するため左側と右側アンカーを選択する (3) 『 I 』 のアンカーを選択する (2) ‘ E’. 参加者のキー入力を記録するだけスクリプト必要がありますまた、応答潜時 (すなわち、各刺激と反応の間の時間) を記録します。 ブロック 1: 最初のターゲット コンセプト差別します。レースはアンカー画面と白の 1 つの側面に黒を表示するセットアップは、反対側に表示されます。特定の方向は被験者間で相殺する必要があります (すなわち被験者の半分必要があります左側の黒と白の右上を見る)。このブロックは 40 試験の合計で構成されています: 後者 20 試験のデータが分析されるに対し、最初の 20 は練習試験。(交換) することがなくランダムにサンプル 50 の顔、それぞれがまさに 2 回 (一度の練習試験、実際の試験に 1 回) 表示されます。各試行の表示を (ランダムに選ばれた各試行) 700 ms や 400 ms、100 ms の間遅延に区切ります。 ブロック 2: 関連付けられた属性の識別。原子価アンカー快適が不快と、画面の片側に表示されるようにする設定は、反対側に表示されます。このブロックは、すべて同じ特性ブロック 1 (すなわち、対抗、試行間の遅延数) 原子価の単語 (例えば, 良い, 悪い) は面ではなく表示されることを除いて。 ブロック 3: 初期結合タスク。(つまり、このブロックの右側にも表示されるし、ブラックがブロック 1 の右側に表示された) 場合、先行するブロックとして同じ方向を使用して両方のレースとヴァランスのアンカーを表示します。40 試験の合計のため、少なくとも 2 回提示各 12 の顔と 12 の原子価の言葉。前のブロックから同じ間遅延を使用します。 ブロック 4: ターゲット コンセプト差別を逆します。ヴァランス アンカーとレース アンカー ブラック ホワイトが表示された画面上に表示され、スワップを削除 (その逆)。それ以外の場合、このブロックには、ブロック 1 のすべての特性が保持されます。 ブロック 5: 逆結合タスク。このブロックは、ブロック 4 にあったレース アンカーは同じ位置に今、ことを除いてブロック 3 と同じです。ブロック 3 と 5 は、参加者間で相殺する必要があります。 すべてのブロック、関連付けられているカテゴリに基づいて、可能な限り迅速に顔/用語を分類する参加者に指示します。 IAT のコンピューター管理タスクを完了すると、レース関連の態度や信念を測定するいくつかのアンケートを配布します。 プライバシーがあることを知っている参加者を可能にするには、個人の実験室でこれらのアンケートを完了するを必要します。かけ、完了したアンケート回答マークされていない封筒に実験者に返す前に知らせるも。 調査は、黒と白の人種概念意味差動策感温度計を含める必要があります-現代人種的優越感のスケール2-だけでなく、多様性と差別のスケール。3 アンカーとして次 – 対極の形容詞対で 5 つのセマンティック ディメンションごとに 7 ポイント スケールを使用:-醜い美しい、良い、悪い、快-不快、正直-不誠実、ニース-ひどい。 これらの 5 つのセマンティックの寸法を使用してすべての 4 つのオブジェクト カテゴリから項目を評価する参加者に指示します。 彼らは、カテゴリに関連する両方のアンカーの形容詞と見なされた場合、スケールの中間をマークする参加者に指示します。 報告: 研究を完了、研究の厳密な性質についての参加者を言います。 3. データ分析 これらの極端な観測は分析を不当に影響しないように、3000 ms 3000 ミリ秒より長い反応時間と同様、300 ms 未満 300 ms 回反応を変換します。 反応時間データが積極的に偏っているので、ログはそれより正常分散されるようにすべての反応時間データを変換します。 ブロック 5 ブロック 3 の平均反応時間を比較します。 IAT の効果のインデックスを計算するこれらのスコアを減算します。 スコア潜在的選好を反映して肯定的な (すなわち、プロ黒) 白と黒、負のスコアを反映 (すなわち、プロの白)、黒対白の潜在的選好によるスコアはゼロを示す黒と白の同等の潜在的選好に対し。 3 (肯定的な) (否定的な)-3 尺度を使用して、各概念の 5 つのディメンションの明示的な評価を平均することによって意味差動スコアを計算します。

Résultats

This procedure typically results in considerably slower responses during Black/pleasant compared to White/pleasant trials (Figure 1). Since slower responses are interpreted to reflect more difficult associations, this longer relative latency (i.e., delay) suggests an implicit attitudinal preference for White over Black. That is, subjects typically find it more challenging to associate Black faces with pleasant nouns. Moreover, when exclusively analyzing responses from White participants, for instance, they often self-report egalitarian preferences (i.e., no preference for either White or Black), despite IAT scores that reveal a strong implicit preference for White over Black (Figure 2).

Figure 1
Figure 1. A typical outcome of the Implicit Association Test. White subjects who performed the Black/pleasant block first. The mean reaction time scores (untransformed) are displayed on the y-axis with error bars equal to one standard deviation. Although the reaction times are log transformed for the analysis, the untransformed scores are displayed for easier interpretation. The x-axis displays the order in which these subjects encountered these blocks. This figure was adapted from Greenwald, McGhee, and Schwartz.1

Figure 2
Figure 2. Relationship of IAT scores to explicit preferences among White participants. The IAT effects scores are displayed on the y-axis with positive scores indicating pro-Black preferences, negative scores indicating pro-White preferences, and zero indicating no differential preference. The semantic differential scores are displayed on the x-axis with positive scores indicating pro-Black preferences, negative scores indicating pro-White preferences, and zero indicating no differential preference. Virtually all White participants that report an explicit pro-Black or egalitarian (i.e., score of zero) semantic preference also show a pro-White preference on the IAT. This figure was adapted from Greenwald, McGhee, and Schwartz.1

Applications and Summary

Since the original paper, the IAT has been extended to examine prejudice in many other domains, such as gender, religion, and sexuality.4 In addition, the IAT has been adapted to (1) dissociate implicit attitudes from stereotypes, (2) measure self-esteem by pairing self/other with pleasant/unpleasant words, and (3) reveal implicit attitudes in children. In some cases, the IAT provides better predictive validity than self-report measures, such as discrimination and suicidal behavior.5

One of the reasons it has become so influential is that it had been made available online at a website called Project Implicit (https://implicit.harvard.edu/implicit/) where anyone can participate in multiple versions. Millions of people have now completed the measure and they have received immediate feedback on how their own implicit preferences compare to other people who have completed the test. The research on implicit bias has had massive implications outside the field of psychology, and implicit bias training is now common in major organizations, governmental agencies, and police departments.

References

  1. Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the Implicit Association Test. Journal of personality and social psychology, 74, 1464.
  2. McConahay, J. B., Hardee, B. B., & Batts, V. (1981). Has racism declined in America? It depends on who is asking and what is asked. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 25, 563-579.
  3. Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. ( 1997). Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 262-274.
  4. Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Hansen, J. J., Devos, T., Lindner, N. M., Ranganath, K. A., Smith, C.T., et al. (2007). Pervasiveness and correlates of implicit attitudes and stereotypes. European Review of Social Psychology, 18, 36-88.
  5. Nock, M. K., Park, J. M., Finn, C. T., Deliberto, T. L., Dour, H. J., & Banaji, M. R. (2010). Measuring the suicidal mind: implicit cognition predicts suicidal behavior. Psychological Science, 21, 511-517.

Transcription

Asking someone to say what’s on their mind can be disconnected from the beliefs that they are willing to discuss or are even knowingly aware of.

While traditional methods often ask individuals to report on their own attitudes—say to evaluate personal feelings about members of a stigmatized group—their opinion is explicit, involving deliberate thought.

Because the topic is sensitive, people are more likely to state unprejudiced, egalitarian views and portray themselves positively even though they may truly harbor negativity.

To circumvent this social-desirability bias, implicit attitudes—evaluations that occur outside of conscious awareness and control—must be examined.

This video demonstrates how to conduct the Implicit Association Test—an influential measure for investigating the strength of associations between a concept like race and automatic evaluations based on the original work of Greenwald and colleagues.

In this experiment, European American participants are recruited to establish out-group homogeneity. They are shown pictures and words—associated with either a particular race or attribute—and asked to quickly and accurately sort them across five different blocks.

The catch is that the stimuli appear in rapid succession—without time for participants to explicitly process them—hence, the name Implicit Association Test.

In the first block of trials, concept discrimination, the initial targets—faces of White European or Black African origin—are randomly presented, without replacement.

If the face shown is White, participants should press the key corresponding to “White”. The first half are considered practice trials, and not saved, as errors are expected while participants get used to responding quickly.

Likewise, for Block 2—attribute discrimination—participants are exposed to only “good” and “bad” words. That is, if “horrible” appears, the correct reply would be a keypress corresponding to “bad”. Thus, the first two blocks serve as baseline latencies associated with correct responses.

During Block 3—the combined portion—either images or words are shown with a race and attribute paired to one response key. Participants must now decide whether the presented face or word like “fabulous” corresponds with “Black or Good” or “White or Bad”.

Block 4—the reversed concept discrimination—is a repeat of Block 1 except that the computer keys for “Black” and “White” are flipped. With this block, participants adjust to the keypresses for the new “Black/White” correspondences.

Finally, Block 5—reversed combined—is similar to Block 3 except that Race is flipped across attributes, such that “Black or Bad” and “White or Good” are now paired on the same response key. Implicit preferences should be observed in latency differences compared to Block 3.

The dependent variable then is the latency to react across block types. Participants are expected to sort faster when good words and White faces go with the same key compared to the opposite, good and Black. Thus, reaction times reveal the strength of each participant’s implicit preference, which agrees with a stereotypical bias.

In addition, the responses can be log-transformed and compared to self-reported attitudes provided on a race-related questionnaire given after the task has been completed.

In this case, if participants’ biases are indeed unspoken, it’s hypothesized that the self-reported beliefs will not correlate to the scores identified during the Implicit Association Test, thus revealing a form of social-desirability.

Before starting the experiment, conduct a power analysis to determine the appropriate number of participants—specifically of European-American descent—that are required.

To begin, greet each one in the lab, explain they will be sorting images and words over several blocks of trials, and have them sign a consent form to participate.

Seat the participant in front of a computer. Further explain that they are to classify, as quickly and accurately as possible, the word or image that appears on the screen by pressing “E” if it fits the category on the left or “I” for the right side. Answer any questions and leave the room.

Start Block 1 by pressing the spacebar. During this initial phase, notice that participants are merely responding to classify faces based on the race anchors Black and White over the course of 100 trials.

Proceeding to Block 2, associated attribute discrimination, observe that now only the list of words and the valence anchors good and bad serve as the classification choices for another 100 trials.

Upon progressing into Block 3, the initial combined task, either images or words appear—but are now paired onto one response key—for a total of 200 trials.

Block 4, reversed target-concept discrimination, is related to Block 1, except that the race anchors now appear on the opposite side for 100 trials.

Finally in Block 5, the reversed combined segment, notice that participants again classify faces and words with combined anchors but the attributes are flipped compared to Block 3.

Following the Implicit Association Test, explain that there are some additional questionnaires to complete on the computer. Emphasize that they will have complete privacy, and then leave the room.

Allow participants enough time to complete the surveys. To conclude, return for debriefing and thank them for taking part in the study.

To visualize the data, plot the mean trial latencies across block type. See the manuscript for details on recoding reaction times.

Notice that responses were slower for the Black/good trials as compared to the White/good ones. Slower responses reflect more difficult associations, suggesting European-American participants found it challenging to associate Black faces with pleasant nouns. In other words, they exhibited an implicit attitudinal preference for the White anchor over the Black anchor.

Also for each participant, compute an index of the Implicit Association Effect by first log transforming the reaction times and then subtracting the means in Block 3 from Block 5. A positive score reflects an automatic preference for Black, whereas a negative one reveals an inclination for White.

Compare these indexes with the semantic differential score calculated by averaging the explicit ratings on the final questionnaire. Here, a value of zero indicates a self-reported egalitarian preference—no race bias.

Results show that most participants did self-report egalitarian preferences despite their IAT scores revealing a moderate to strong implicit preference for White over Black. These results suggest that a social-desirability bias may have distorted their responses to the questionnaire.

Now that you are familiar with how the Implicit Association Test can examine automatic bias and prejudice for socially sensitive topics, let’s look at other real-life situations where the task can be applied.

Researchers have published the Implicit Association Test on the internet allowing anyone to participate, on a number of topics, including social attitudes and mental health. Participants receive immediate feedback on their implicit preferences, providing a straight-forward approach for examining their own beliefs and biases in the comfort of their own home or office.

Other researchers have used the task to measure self-esteem by pairing self/other with pleasant/unpleasant words. These tests can see through any fa¸ade or deception leading to better success with interventions and treatment.

Lastly, children are notoriously difficult to get reliable information from, especially when asked to self-report measures. For this reason, researchers use Implicit Association Tests to assess young attitudes and beliefs, ranging from versions that investigate preferences towards race, gender, and even healthy-eating behaviors.

You’ve just watched JoVE’s video on the Implicit Association Test. Now you should have a good understanding of how to design and execute an experiment using Implicit Association tests, how to analyze and assess the results, as well as how to apply the principles to a number of real-world situations.

Thanks for watching!

Tags

Cite This
JoVE Science Education Database. Education. The Implicit Association Test. JoVE, Cambridge, MA, (2023).