JoVE Science Education
Social Psychology
This content is Free Access.
JoVE Science Education Social Psychology
The Implicit Association Test
  • 00:00Vue d'ensemble
  • 01:19Experimental Design
  • 04:32Running the Experiment
  • 06:49Representative Results
  • 08:29Applications
  • 09:45Summary

암시적 연관 검사

English

Diviser

Vue d'ensemble

출처: 줄리안 윌스 & 제이 반 바벨-뉴욕 대학교

사회 심리학의 핵심 구조 중 하나는 객체 또는 사람에 대한 태도의 개념입니다. 전통적으로 심리학자들은 사람들에게 자신의 믿음, 의견 또는 감정을 스스로 보고하도록 요청함으로써 태도를 측정했습니다. 그러나 이 접근법은 인종적 편견과 같은 사회적으로 민감한 태도를 측정할 때, 사람들이 종종 편견없는 평등주의적 신념을 스스로 보고하도록 동기를 부여받기 때문에(부정적인 연관성을 품고 있음에도 불구하고) 한계가 있습니다. 이러한 사회적 바람직성 편향을 우회하기 위해 심리학자들은 의도적인 통제(및 잠재적 왜곡)에 덜 순종하는 암시적 태도를 측정하기 위한 여러 가지 작업을 개발했습니다.

암시적 협회 테스트( IAT)는 이러한 무의식적인 태도중 가장 영향력 있는 조치 중 하나입니다. IAT는 앤서니 그린발트와 동료들이 1998년 논문에 처음 도입했습니다. 1 이 비디오는 최종 실험에서 사용되는 IAT를 수행하는 방법을 보여 주며, 유럽계 미국인 참가자(명시적 평등주의적 태도를 보고함)는 자신의 인종에 대한 암시적 선호도를 나타낸다.

Procédure

1. 참가자 모집 전력 분석을 수행하고 관찰된 효과 크기를 감지한 다음 정보에 입각한 동의를 얻기에 충분한 통계적 힘을 얻을 수 있는 충분한 참가자를 모집합니다. 2. 데이터 수집 유쾌한 12 단어의 목록을 조립 – 좋은 – 협회(예 :,행복, 행운, 선물) 불쾌한 – 나쁜 – 협회(예 :증오, 재해, 독)와 12 단어. 12 명의 유럽계 미국인과 12 명의 아프리카 계 미국인 얼굴 (절반 남성, 절반 여성)을 턱과 이마에 자른 다. 5개의블록(즉,시퀀스)으로 자극 프레젠테이션 스크립트를 만듭니다. 스크립트는 (1) 진행 지시에 대한 ‘SPACEBAR’, (2) ‘E’가 왼쪽의 앵커를 선택하고 (3) 오른쪽에 있는 앵커를 선택하는 세 개의 키보드 응답만 허용해야 합니다. 참가자의 주요 누액을 기록하는 것 외에도 스크립트는 응답 대기시간(즉,제공된 각 자극 프레젠테이션 및 응답 사이의 시간)을 기록해야 합니다. 블록 1: 초기 대상 개념 차별. 화면 한쪽에 검은색이 표시되고 흰색이 반대쪽에 표시되도록 레이스 앵커를 설정합니다. 특정 방향은 피사체 간에 균형을 유지해야합니다(즉,피험자의 절반은 왼쪽에 검은색을, 오른쪽에 흰색을 보아야 합니다). 이 블록은 총 40개의 시험으로 구성됩니다: 처음 20개는 연습 시험인 반면, 데이터는 후자의 20번의 시험에서 분석됩니다. 무작위로 샘플링 (교체없이) 각각정확히 두 번 표시되도록 (연습 시험을 위해 한 번, 실제 시험을 위해 한 번). 100 ms, 400 ms 또는 700 ms (무작위로 각 시험 선택)의 재판 간 지연에 의해 각 재판의 표시를 분리합니다. 블록 2: 연관된 특성 차별. 즐거운 화면의 한쪽에 표시하고 불쾌한 반대쪽에 표시되도록 valence 앵커를 설정합니다. 이 블록은 블록 1(즉,카운터 밸런싱, 시험 횟수, 재판 간 지연)과 동일한 특성을 가지며, 발렌스 단어(예 :좋은, 나쁜)가 얼굴 대신 표시됩니다. 블록 3: 초기 결합 작업입니다. 앞의 블록과 동일한 방향을 사용하여 레이스 및 원자 앵커를 모두 표시합니다(예: Black이 블록 1의 오른쪽에 표시되는 경우 이 블록의 오른쪽에 표시되어야 합니다). 총 40번의 시련을 위해 12개의 얼굴과 12개의 용감한 단어를 각각 두 번 이상 제시한다. 이전 블록에서 동일한 평가판 간 지연을 사용합니다. 블록 4: 대상 개념 차별을 역전시켰습니다. 원자 앵커를 제거하고 레이스 앵커를 교체하여 이제 Black이 원래 표시된 화면에 표시됩니다(그리고 그 반대의 경우도 마찬가지입니다). 그렇지 않으면 이 블록은 블록 1의 모든 특성을 유지합니다. 블록 5: 결합된 작업을 번복합니다. 이 블록은 레이스 앵커가 블록 4에 있던 것과 동일한 위치에 있다는 점을 제외하면 블록 3과 동일합니다. 블록 3과 5는 참가자 간에 균형을 유지해야 합니다. 모든 블록의 경우 참가자에게 연결된 범주에 따라 얼굴/용어를 가능한 한 빨리 분류하도록 지시합니다. 컴퓨터 로 관리되는 IAT 작업을 완료한 후 인종 관련 태도와 신념을 측정하는 여러 설문지를 배포합니다. 참가자가 개인 정보 보호가 있다는 것을 알 수 있도록 개인 실험실에서 이러한 설문지를 작성하도록 하십시오. 또한 완성된 설문지를 표시되지 않은 봉투에 넣은 후 실험자에게 돌려보내겠다고 알려드립니다. 설문 조사에는 흑인과 백인의 인종 개념인 현대 인종차별주의 척도2를대상으로 한 느낌 온도계와 의미론적 차등 측정과 다양성과 차별 척도가 포함되어야 합니다. 3 앵커로 다음과 같은 극성 형용사 쌍과 다섯 의미 적 차원의 각각에 대한 7 점 척도를 사용 : 아름다운 – 못생긴, 좋은 나쁜, 즐거운 – 불쾌한, 정직 – 부정직, 좋은 – 끔찍한. 참가자에게 이 다섯 가지 의미 체계 치수를 사용하여 네 가지 개체 범주의 항목을 평가하도록 지시합니다. 참가자들에게 두 앵커링 형용사가 범주와 무관하다고 판단한 경우 축척 의 중간을 표시하도록 지시합니다. 브리핑: 연구를 완료하려면 참가자에게 연구의 정확한 특성에 대해 알려주십시오. 3. 데이터 분석 300ms에서 300ms까지 300ms 미만의 반응 횟수와 3000ms에 3000ms 이상의 반응 시간을 다시 코딩하여 이러한 극단적인 관찰이 분석에 지나치게 영향을 미치지 않도록 합니다. 반응 시간 데이터가 긍정적으로 왜곡되기 때문에 로그는 모든 반응 시간 데이터를 변환하여 보다 정상적으로 배포되도록 합니다. 그런 다음 블록 3의 평균 반응 시간을 블록 5와 비교합니다. 이러한 점수를 빼고 IAT 효과에 대한 인덱스를 계산합니다. 긍정 점수는흑백(즉,프로-블랙)에 대한 암시적 선호도를 반영하며, 음수 점수는 흰색 대 블랙(예: 프로-화이트)에 대한 암시적 선호도를 반영하는 반면, 0점수는 흑백에 대한 암묵적 선호도를 나타냅니다. -3(음수)에서 3(양수)까지의 척도를 사용하여 각 개념의 5차원에 대한 명시적 등급을 평균화하여 의미 체계 차등 점수를 계산합니다.

Résultats

This procedure typically results in considerably slower responses during Black/pleasant compared to White/pleasant trials (Figure 1). Since slower responses are interpreted to reflect more difficult associations, this longer relative latency (i.e., delay) suggests an implicit attitudinal preference for White over Black. That is, subjects typically find it more challenging to associate Black faces with pleasant nouns. Moreover, when exclusively analyzing responses from White participants, for instance, they often self-report egalitarian preferences (i.e., no preference for either White or Black), despite IAT scores that reveal a strong implicit preference for White over Black (Figure 2).

Figure 1
Figure 1. A typical outcome of the Implicit Association Test. White subjects who performed the Black/pleasant block first. The mean reaction time scores (untransformed) are displayed on the y-axis with error bars equal to one standard deviation. Although the reaction times are log transformed for the analysis, the untransformed scores are displayed for easier interpretation. The x-axis displays the order in which these subjects encountered these blocks. This figure was adapted from Greenwald, McGhee, and Schwartz.1

Figure 2
Figure 2. Relationship of IAT scores to explicit preferences among White participants. The IAT effects scores are displayed on the y-axis with positive scores indicating pro-Black preferences, negative scores indicating pro-White preferences, and zero indicating no differential preference. The semantic differential scores are displayed on the x-axis with positive scores indicating pro-Black preferences, negative scores indicating pro-White preferences, and zero indicating no differential preference. Virtually all White participants that report an explicit pro-Black or egalitarian (i.e., score of zero) semantic preference also show a pro-White preference on the IAT. This figure was adapted from Greenwald, McGhee, and Schwartz.1

Applications and Summary

Since the original paper, the IAT has been extended to examine prejudice in many other domains, such as gender, religion, and sexuality.4 In addition, the IAT has been adapted to (1) dissociate implicit attitudes from stereotypes, (2) measure self-esteem by pairing self/other with pleasant/unpleasant words, and (3) reveal implicit attitudes in children. In some cases, the IAT provides better predictive validity than self-report measures, such as discrimination and suicidal behavior.5

One of the reasons it has become so influential is that it had been made available online at a website called Project Implicit (https://implicit.harvard.edu/implicit/) where anyone can participate in multiple versions. Millions of people have now completed the measure and they have received immediate feedback on how their own implicit preferences compare to other people who have completed the test. The research on implicit bias has had massive implications outside the field of psychology, and implicit bias training is now common in major organizations, governmental agencies, and police departments.

References

  1. Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the Implicit Association Test. Journal of personality and social psychology, 74, 1464.
  2. McConahay, J. B., Hardee, B. B., & Batts, V. (1981). Has racism declined in America? It depends on who is asking and what is asked. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 25, 563-579.
  3. Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. ( 1997). Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 262-274.
  4. Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Hansen, J. J., Devos, T., Lindner, N. M., Ranganath, K. A., Smith, C.T., et al. (2007). Pervasiveness and correlates of implicit attitudes and stereotypes. European Review of Social Psychology, 18, 36-88.
  5. Nock, M. K., Park, J. M., Finn, C. T., Deliberto, T. L., Dour, H. J., & Banaji, M. R. (2010). Measuring the suicidal mind: implicit cognition predicts suicidal behavior. Psychological Science, 21, 511-517.

Transcription

Asking someone to say what’s on their mind can be disconnected from the beliefs that they are willing to discuss or are even knowingly aware of.

While traditional methods often ask individuals to report on their own attitudes—say to evaluate personal feelings about members of a stigmatized group—their opinion is explicit, involving deliberate thought.

Because the topic is sensitive, people are more likely to state unprejudiced, egalitarian views and portray themselves positively even though they may truly harbor negativity.

To circumvent this social-desirability bias, implicit attitudes—evaluations that occur outside of conscious awareness and control—must be examined.

This video demonstrates how to conduct the Implicit Association Test—an influential measure for investigating the strength of associations between a concept like race and automatic evaluations based on the original work of Greenwald and colleagues.

In this experiment, European American participants are recruited to establish out-group homogeneity. They are shown pictures and words—associated with either a particular race or attribute—and asked to quickly and accurately sort them across five different blocks.

The catch is that the stimuli appear in rapid succession—without time for participants to explicitly process them—hence, the name Implicit Association Test.

In the first block of trials, concept discrimination, the initial targets—faces of White European or Black African origin—are randomly presented, without replacement.

If the face shown is White, participants should press the key corresponding to “White”. The first half are considered practice trials, and not saved, as errors are expected while participants get used to responding quickly.

Likewise, for Block 2—attribute discrimination—participants are exposed to only “good” and “bad” words. That is, if “horrible” appears, the correct reply would be a keypress corresponding to “bad”. Thus, the first two blocks serve as baseline latencies associated with correct responses.

During Block 3—the combined portion—either images or words are shown with a race and attribute paired to one response key. Participants must now decide whether the presented face or word like “fabulous” corresponds with “Black or Good” or “White or Bad”.

Block 4—the reversed concept discrimination—is a repeat of Block 1 except that the computer keys for “Black” and “White” are flipped. With this block, participants adjust to the keypresses for the new “Black/White” correspondences.

Finally, Block 5—reversed combined—is similar to Block 3 except that Race is flipped across attributes, such that “Black or Bad” and “White or Good” are now paired on the same response key. Implicit preferences should be observed in latency differences compared to Block 3.

The dependent variable then is the latency to react across block types. Participants are expected to sort faster when good words and White faces go with the same key compared to the opposite, good and Black. Thus, reaction times reveal the strength of each participant’s implicit preference, which agrees with a stereotypical bias.

In addition, the responses can be log-transformed and compared to self-reported attitudes provided on a race-related questionnaire given after the task has been completed.

In this case, if participants’ biases are indeed unspoken, it’s hypothesized that the self-reported beliefs will not correlate to the scores identified during the Implicit Association Test, thus revealing a form of social-desirability.

Before starting the experiment, conduct a power analysis to determine the appropriate number of participants—specifically of European-American descent—that are required.

To begin, greet each one in the lab, explain they will be sorting images and words over several blocks of trials, and have them sign a consent form to participate.

Seat the participant in front of a computer. Further explain that they are to classify, as quickly and accurately as possible, the word or image that appears on the screen by pressing “E” if it fits the category on the left or “I” for the right side. Answer any questions and leave the room.

Start Block 1 by pressing the spacebar. During this initial phase, notice that participants are merely responding to classify faces based on the race anchors Black and White over the course of 100 trials.

Proceeding to Block 2, associated attribute discrimination, observe that now only the list of words and the valence anchors good and bad serve as the classification choices for another 100 trials.

Upon progressing into Block 3, the initial combined task, either images or words appear—but are now paired onto one response key—for a total of 200 trials.

Block 4, reversed target-concept discrimination, is related to Block 1, except that the race anchors now appear on the opposite side for 100 trials.

Finally in Block 5, the reversed combined segment, notice that participants again classify faces and words with combined anchors but the attributes are flipped compared to Block 3.

Following the Implicit Association Test, explain that there are some additional questionnaires to complete on the computer. Emphasize that they will have complete privacy, and then leave the room.

Allow participants enough time to complete the surveys. To conclude, return for debriefing and thank them for taking part in the study.

To visualize the data, plot the mean trial latencies across block type. See the manuscript for details on recoding reaction times.

Notice that responses were slower for the Black/good trials as compared to the White/good ones. Slower responses reflect more difficult associations, suggesting European-American participants found it challenging to associate Black faces with pleasant nouns. In other words, they exhibited an implicit attitudinal preference for the White anchor over the Black anchor.

Also for each participant, compute an index of the Implicit Association Effect by first log transforming the reaction times and then subtracting the means in Block 3 from Block 5. A positive score reflects an automatic preference for Black, whereas a negative one reveals an inclination for White.

Compare these indexes with the semantic differential score calculated by averaging the explicit ratings on the final questionnaire. Here, a value of zero indicates a self-reported egalitarian preference—no race bias.

Results show that most participants did self-report egalitarian preferences despite their IAT scores revealing a moderate to strong implicit preference for White over Black. These results suggest that a social-desirability bias may have distorted their responses to the questionnaire.

Now that you are familiar with how the Implicit Association Test can examine automatic bias and prejudice for socially sensitive topics, let’s look at other real-life situations where the task can be applied.

Researchers have published the Implicit Association Test on the internet allowing anyone to participate, on a number of topics, including social attitudes and mental health. Participants receive immediate feedback on their implicit preferences, providing a straight-forward approach for examining their own beliefs and biases in the comfort of their own home or office.

Other researchers have used the task to measure self-esteem by pairing self/other with pleasant/unpleasant words. These tests can see through any fa¸ade or deception leading to better success with interventions and treatment.

Lastly, children are notoriously difficult to get reliable information from, especially when asked to self-report measures. For this reason, researchers use Implicit Association Tests to assess young attitudes and beliefs, ranging from versions that investigate preferences towards race, gender, and even healthy-eating behaviors.

You’ve just watched JoVE’s video on the Implicit Association Test. Now you should have a good understanding of how to design and execute an experiment using Implicit Association tests, how to analyze and assess the results, as well as how to apply the principles to a number of real-world situations.

Thanks for watching!

Tags

Cite This
JoVE Science Education Database. Education. The Implicit Association Test. JoVE, Cambridge, MA, (2023).