JoVE Science Education
Social Psychology
This content is Free Access.
JoVE Science Education Social Psychology
The Implicit Association Test
  • 00:00Vue d'ensemble
  • 01:19Experimental Design
  • 04:32Running the Experiment
  • 06:49Representative Results
  • 08:29Applications
  • 09:45Summary

O teste de associação implícita

English

Diviser

Vue d'ensemble

Fonte: Julian Wills & Jay Van Bavel — Universidade de Nova York

Um dos núcleos da psicologia social é a noção de atitude em relação a um objeto ou pessoa. Tradicionalmente, os psicólogos mediam atitudes simplesmente pedindo às pessoas que auto-relatassem suas crenças, opiniões ou sentimentos. Essa abordagem tem limitações, no entanto, ao medir atitudes socialmente sensíveis, como o preconceito racial, porque muitas vezes as pessoas são motivadas a auto-relatar crenças não prejudianas e igualitárias (apesar de abrigarem associações negativas). Para contornar esse viés de desejo social, os psicólogos desenvolveram uma série de tarefas que tentam medir atitudes implícitas menos favoráveis ao controle deliberado (e potencial distorção).

O Teste de Associação Implícita, ou IAT, é uma das medidas mais influentes dessas atitudes inconscientes. O IAT foi introduzido pela primeira vez em um artigo de 1998 por Anthony Greenwald e colegas. 1 Este vídeo demonstrará como conduzir o IAT usado no experimento final, onde os participantes europeus americanos (que relatam atitudes igualitárias explícitas) exibem preferências implícitas para sua própria raça.

Procédure

1. Recrutamento de Participantes Realize uma análise de poder e recrute participantes suficientes para obter poder estatístico suficiente para detectar o tamanho do efeito observado e, em seguida, obter consentimento informado. 2. Coleta de Dados Monte uma lista de 12 palavras com associações agradáveis – boas – (por exemplo,feliz, sorte, dom) e 12 palavras com associações desagradáveis – ruins – (por exemplo,ódio, desastre, veneno). Monte 12 rostos europeus americanos e 12 afro-americanos (metade masculino, metade feminino), cortados no queixo e na testa. Crie um script de apresentação de estímulo com cinco blocos (ou seja,, sequências). O script só deve permitir três respostas do teclado: (1) ‘SPACEBAR’ para o avanço das instruções, (2) ‘E’ para selecionar âncoras à esquerda e (3) ‘I’ para selecionar âncoras à direita. Além de gravar as principais prensas do participante, o script também deve registrar sua latência de resposta (ou seja,tempo entre cada apresentação de estímulo e resposta fornecida). Bloco 1: Discriminação de conceito-alvo inicial. Configure as âncoras de corrida para que o Preto seja exibido em um lado da tela e o Branco seja exibido no lado oposto. A orientação específica deve ser contrabalanceada entre os sujeitos (ou seja,metade dos sujeitos deve ver Preto à esquerda e Branco à direita). Este bloco consiste em 40 ensaios no total: Os primeiros 20 são ensaios práticos, enquanto os dados são analisados nos últimos 20 ensaios. Amostra aleatoriamente (sem substituição) as cinquenta faces para que cada uma seja exibida exatamente duas vezes (uma para ensaios práticos, uma vez para ensaios reais). Separe a exibição de cada ensaio por um atraso entre julgamentos de 100 ms, 400 ms ou 700 ms (escolhido aleatoriamente cada ensaio). Bloco 2: Discriminação de atributos associados. Configure as âncoras de valência para que agradável seja exibido em um lado da tela e desagradável seja exibido no lado oposto. Este bloco tem todas as mesmas características do Bloco 1 (ou seja,contrabalança, número de ensaios, atraso entre julgamentos) exceto que as palavras de valência (por exemplo,boas, ruins) são exibidas em vez de faces. Bloco 3: Tarefa combinada inicial. Exibir tanto as âncoras de raça quanto de valência usando a mesma orientação dos blocos anteriores (ou seja, se black foi exibido à direita para o Bloco 1, então ele também deve aparecer à direita para este bloco). Apresente cada uma das 12 faces e 12 palavras de valência pelo menos duas vezes, para um total de 40 ensaios. Use os mesmos atrasos entre ensaios dos blocos anteriores. Bloco 4: Discriminação de conceito-alvo invertido. Remova as âncoras de valência e troque as âncoras de corrida para que o Preto seja agora exibido na tela onde o Branco foi exibido originalmente (e vice-versa). Caso contrário, este bloco mantém todas as características do Bloco 1. Bloco 5: Tarefa combinada invertida. Este bloco é idêntico ao Bloco 3, exceto que as âncoras de corrida estão agora na mesma posição que estavam no Bloco 4. Os blocos 3 e 5 devem ser contrabalançados entre os participantes. Para todos os blocos, instrua os participantes a classificar os faces/termos o mais rápido possível, com base na categoria a que estão associados. Após o preenchimento das tarefas de IAT administradas por computador, distribua vários questionários que medem atitudes e crenças relacionadas à raça. Para permitir que os participantes saibam que têm privacidade, que eles completem esses questionários em salas experimentais pessoais. Informe-os também que colocarão seus questionários preenchidos em um envelope não marcado antes de devolvê-los ao experimentador. As pesquisas devem incluir um termômetro de sentimento e medidas diferenciais semânticas voltadas para os conceitos raciais de Preto e Branco — a Escala Moderna do Racismo2— bem como as Escalas de Diversidade e Discriminação. 3 Use escalas de 7 pontos para cada uma das cinco dimensões semânticas com os seguintes pares de adjetivos polares opostos como âncoras: bonito-feio, bom-ruim, agradável-desagradável, honesto-desonesto, agradável-horrível. Instrua os participantes a classificar itens de todas as quatro categorias de objetos usando essas cinco dimensões semânticas. Instrua os participantes a marcar o meio da escala se considerarem que ambos ancoram adjetivos são irrelevantes para a categoria. Debriefing: Para concluir o estudo, informe o participante sobre a natureza exata do estudo. 3. Análise de dados Recodificar vezes menos de 300 ms a 300 ms, bem como tempos de reação maiores que 3000 ms a 3000 ms, de modo que essas observações extremas não influenciam indevidamente a análise. Como os dados do tempo de reação são positivamente distorcidos, o log transforma todos os dados de tempos de reação para que sejam mais normalmente distribuídos. Em seguida, compare os tempos médios de reação no Bloco 3 com o Bloco 5. Subtraia essas pontuações para calcular um índice para o efeito IAT. Os resultados positivos refletem uma preferência implícita por Preto versus Branco (ou seja,pró-Preto), os resultados negativos refletem uma preferência implícita por Branco versus Preto(ou seja,pró-Branco), enquanto uma pontuação de zero indica preferência implícita equivalente para Preto e Branco. Calcular um escore diferencial semântico, com média de classificações explícitas para as cinco dimensões de cada conceito, utilizando uma escala de -3 (negativo) a 3 (positivo).

Résultats

This procedure typically results in considerably slower responses during Black/pleasant compared to White/pleasant trials (Figure 1). Since slower responses are interpreted to reflect more difficult associations, this longer relative latency (i.e., delay) suggests an implicit attitudinal preference for White over Black. That is, subjects typically find it more challenging to associate Black faces with pleasant nouns. Moreover, when exclusively analyzing responses from White participants, for instance, they often self-report egalitarian preferences (i.e., no preference for either White or Black), despite IAT scores that reveal a strong implicit preference for White over Black (Figure 2).

Figure 1
Figure 1. A typical outcome of the Implicit Association Test. White subjects who performed the Black/pleasant block first. The mean reaction time scores (untransformed) are displayed on the y-axis with error bars equal to one standard deviation. Although the reaction times are log transformed for the analysis, the untransformed scores are displayed for easier interpretation. The x-axis displays the order in which these subjects encountered these blocks. This figure was adapted from Greenwald, McGhee, and Schwartz.1

Figure 2
Figure 2. Relationship of IAT scores to explicit preferences among White participants. The IAT effects scores are displayed on the y-axis with positive scores indicating pro-Black preferences, negative scores indicating pro-White preferences, and zero indicating no differential preference. The semantic differential scores are displayed on the x-axis with positive scores indicating pro-Black preferences, negative scores indicating pro-White preferences, and zero indicating no differential preference. Virtually all White participants that report an explicit pro-Black or egalitarian (i.e., score of zero) semantic preference also show a pro-White preference on the IAT. This figure was adapted from Greenwald, McGhee, and Schwartz.1

Applications and Summary

Since the original paper, the IAT has been extended to examine prejudice in many other domains, such as gender, religion, and sexuality.4 In addition, the IAT has been adapted to (1) dissociate implicit attitudes from stereotypes, (2) measure self-esteem by pairing self/other with pleasant/unpleasant words, and (3) reveal implicit attitudes in children. In some cases, the IAT provides better predictive validity than self-report measures, such as discrimination and suicidal behavior.5

One of the reasons it has become so influential is that it had been made available online at a website called Project Implicit (https://implicit.harvard.edu/implicit/) where anyone can participate in multiple versions. Millions of people have now completed the measure and they have received immediate feedback on how their own implicit preferences compare to other people who have completed the test. The research on implicit bias has had massive implications outside the field of psychology, and implicit bias training is now common in major organizations, governmental agencies, and police departments.

References

  1. Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the Implicit Association Test. Journal of personality and social psychology, 74, 1464.
  2. McConahay, J. B., Hardee, B. B., & Batts, V. (1981). Has racism declined in America? It depends on who is asking and what is asked. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 25, 563-579.
  3. Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. ( 1997). Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 262-274.
  4. Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Hansen, J. J., Devos, T., Lindner, N. M., Ranganath, K. A., Smith, C.T., et al. (2007). Pervasiveness and correlates of implicit attitudes and stereotypes. European Review of Social Psychology, 18, 36-88.
  5. Nock, M. K., Park, J. M., Finn, C. T., Deliberto, T. L., Dour, H. J., & Banaji, M. R. (2010). Measuring the suicidal mind: implicit cognition predicts suicidal behavior. Psychological Science, 21, 511-517.

Transcription

Asking someone to say what’s on their mind can be disconnected from the beliefs that they are willing to discuss or are even knowingly aware of.

While traditional methods often ask individuals to report on their own attitudes—say to evaluate personal feelings about members of a stigmatized group—their opinion is explicit, involving deliberate thought.

Because the topic is sensitive, people are more likely to state unprejudiced, egalitarian views and portray themselves positively even though they may truly harbor negativity.

To circumvent this social-desirability bias, implicit attitudes—evaluations that occur outside of conscious awareness and control—must be examined.

This video demonstrates how to conduct the Implicit Association Test—an influential measure for investigating the strength of associations between a concept like race and automatic evaluations based on the original work of Greenwald and colleagues.

In this experiment, European American participants are recruited to establish out-group homogeneity. They are shown pictures and words—associated with either a particular race or attribute—and asked to quickly and accurately sort them across five different blocks.

The catch is that the stimuli appear in rapid succession—without time for participants to explicitly process them—hence, the name Implicit Association Test.

In the first block of trials, concept discrimination, the initial targets—faces of White European or Black African origin—are randomly presented, without replacement.

If the face shown is White, participants should press the key corresponding to “White”. The first half are considered practice trials, and not saved, as errors are expected while participants get used to responding quickly.

Likewise, for Block 2—attribute discrimination—participants are exposed to only “good” and “bad” words. That is, if “horrible” appears, the correct reply would be a keypress corresponding to “bad”. Thus, the first two blocks serve as baseline latencies associated with correct responses.

During Block 3—the combined portion—either images or words are shown with a race and attribute paired to one response key. Participants must now decide whether the presented face or word like “fabulous” corresponds with “Black or Good” or “White or Bad”.

Block 4—the reversed concept discrimination—is a repeat of Block 1 except that the computer keys for “Black” and “White” are flipped. With this block, participants adjust to the keypresses for the new “Black/White” correspondences.

Finally, Block 5—reversed combined—is similar to Block 3 except that Race is flipped across attributes, such that “Black or Bad” and “White or Good” are now paired on the same response key. Implicit preferences should be observed in latency differences compared to Block 3.

The dependent variable then is the latency to react across block types. Participants are expected to sort faster when good words and White faces go with the same key compared to the opposite, good and Black. Thus, reaction times reveal the strength of each participant’s implicit preference, which agrees with a stereotypical bias.

In addition, the responses can be log-transformed and compared to self-reported attitudes provided on a race-related questionnaire given after the task has been completed.

In this case, if participants’ biases are indeed unspoken, it’s hypothesized that the self-reported beliefs will not correlate to the scores identified during the Implicit Association Test, thus revealing a form of social-desirability.

Before starting the experiment, conduct a power analysis to determine the appropriate number of participants—specifically of European-American descent—that are required.

To begin, greet each one in the lab, explain they will be sorting images and words over several blocks of trials, and have them sign a consent form to participate.

Seat the participant in front of a computer. Further explain that they are to classify, as quickly and accurately as possible, the word or image that appears on the screen by pressing “E” if it fits the category on the left or “I” for the right side. Answer any questions and leave the room.

Start Block 1 by pressing the spacebar. During this initial phase, notice that participants are merely responding to classify faces based on the race anchors Black and White over the course of 100 trials.

Proceeding to Block 2, associated attribute discrimination, observe that now only the list of words and the valence anchors good and bad serve as the classification choices for another 100 trials.

Upon progressing into Block 3, the initial combined task, either images or words appear—but are now paired onto one response key—for a total of 200 trials.

Block 4, reversed target-concept discrimination, is related to Block 1, except that the race anchors now appear on the opposite side for 100 trials.

Finally in Block 5, the reversed combined segment, notice that participants again classify faces and words with combined anchors but the attributes are flipped compared to Block 3.

Following the Implicit Association Test, explain that there are some additional questionnaires to complete on the computer. Emphasize that they will have complete privacy, and then leave the room.

Allow participants enough time to complete the surveys. To conclude, return for debriefing and thank them for taking part in the study.

To visualize the data, plot the mean trial latencies across block type. See the manuscript for details on recoding reaction times.

Notice that responses were slower for the Black/good trials as compared to the White/good ones. Slower responses reflect more difficult associations, suggesting European-American participants found it challenging to associate Black faces with pleasant nouns. In other words, they exhibited an implicit attitudinal preference for the White anchor over the Black anchor.

Also for each participant, compute an index of the Implicit Association Effect by first log transforming the reaction times and then subtracting the means in Block 3 from Block 5. A positive score reflects an automatic preference for Black, whereas a negative one reveals an inclination for White.

Compare these indexes with the semantic differential score calculated by averaging the explicit ratings on the final questionnaire. Here, a value of zero indicates a self-reported egalitarian preference—no race bias.

Results show that most participants did self-report egalitarian preferences despite their IAT scores revealing a moderate to strong implicit preference for White over Black. These results suggest that a social-desirability bias may have distorted their responses to the questionnaire.

Now that you are familiar with how the Implicit Association Test can examine automatic bias and prejudice for socially sensitive topics, let’s look at other real-life situations where the task can be applied.

Researchers have published the Implicit Association Test on the internet allowing anyone to participate, on a number of topics, including social attitudes and mental health. Participants receive immediate feedback on their implicit preferences, providing a straight-forward approach for examining their own beliefs and biases in the comfort of their own home or office.

Other researchers have used the task to measure self-esteem by pairing self/other with pleasant/unpleasant words. These tests can see through any fa¸ade or deception leading to better success with interventions and treatment.

Lastly, children are notoriously difficult to get reliable information from, especially when asked to self-report measures. For this reason, researchers use Implicit Association Tests to assess young attitudes and beliefs, ranging from versions that investigate preferences towards race, gender, and even healthy-eating behaviors.

You’ve just watched JoVE’s video on the Implicit Association Test. Now you should have a good understanding of how to design and execute an experiment using Implicit Association tests, how to analyze and assess the results, as well as how to apply the principles to a number of real-world situations.

Thanks for watching!

Tags

Cite This
JoVE Science Education Database. Education. The Implicit Association Test. JoVE, Cambridge, MA, (2023).